Skip to main content
Log in

Change Processes During Cognitive Bias Modification for Obsessive Compulsive Beliefs

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Multiple studies have found that cognitive bias modification (CBM) can be an effective intervention to reduce maladaptive, anxiety-linked cognitive biases and symptoms, but little is known about how it achieves its effects. CBM is posited to work by altering contingency learning about potential threat cues, rather than via habituation of fear and arousal (as hypothesized for exposure-based interventions). In the current study, multi-level modeling was used to examine the trajectories of potential change processes over the course of CBM for interpretation bias in a sample high in obsessive–compulsive (OC) symptoms (n = 75). Psychophysiological arousal (heart rate and galvanic skin response), subjective fear, and the development of a learned contingency between ambiguity tied to threat and safe outcomes were measured before, during, and after CBM. Results showed that, compared to a control group, CBM was effective at reducing OC beliefs (though not responses to subsequent OC stressors). Additionally, as expected, only contingency learning significantly changed as a function of training condition, while subjective fear and arousal did not. Moreover, post-training indicators of contingency learning predicted the extent OC beliefs changed from pre- to post-training for the “positive” CBM group only (and not for the control group). This indicates that CBM is likely not operating similarly to habituation, but is instead marked by change in cognitive processing. However, change in contingency learning did not fully mediate the effect of this intervention on change in OC beliefs, suggesting more work is needed to fully understand the mechanisms underlying CBM.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The recognition ratings task often also includes disorder-irrelevant interpretations to serve as “foils.” These foil options were not included in the current task to save time given the repeated administration of the recognition ratings task.

  2. All covariates were converted to z-scores in order to apply a constant transformation so that they were all on the same metric.

  3. If the two measures with baseline differences (negative interpretations, and depression) and the six subscale means of the OCI-R are not included as covariates in the analysis, the interaction does not reach significance (F(1, 65) = 1.96, p = .166, η 2 p  = .03).

  4. For the multi-level modeling procedure and the structural regression analyses, a constant of one was added to the EDA variable to obtain non-negative numbers, and EDA was then log-transformed to reduce positive skew in order to maximize a normal distribution.

  5. Only three participants who received the funnel debriefing reported suspicion related to the deceptive components of the tasks. In the interest of maximizing power, all participants were included in the analyses. Additionally, performance on the dichotomous pen-cleaning variable was not included in our analyses because its inclusion prevented convergence of the models.

  6. We thank an anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.

References

  • Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Whitehouse, W. G., Hogan, M. E., Tashman, N. A., Steinberg, D. L., et al. (1999). Depressogenic cognitive styles: Predictive validity, information processing and personality characteristics, and developmental origins. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 503–531. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00157-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aronson, E., Ellsworth, P. C., Carlsmith, J. M., & Gonzales, M. H. (1990). Methods of research in social psychology (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, A., Mystkowski, J., Culver, N., Yi, R., Mortazavi, A., & Craske, M. G. (2010). Does habituation matter? Emotional processing theory and exposure therapy for acrophobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 1139–1143. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.009.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beard, C., Sawyer, A. T., & Hofmann, S. G. (2012). Efficacy of attention bias modification using threat and appetitive stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Behavior Therapy, 43, 724–740. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2012.01.002.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1997). An information processing model of anxiety: Automatic and strategic processes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 49–58. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00069-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belloch, A., Morillo, C., & Garcia-Soriano, G. (2007). Are the dysfunctional beliefs that predict worry different from those that predict obsessions? Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 14, 438–448. doi:10.1002/cpp.551.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1990). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In A. Healy & R. Shiffrin (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (2nd ed., pp. 35–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinaru, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.). (2007). Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calleo, J. S., Hart, J., Bjorgvinsson, T., & Stanley, M. A. (2010). Obsessions and worry beliefs in an inpatient OCD population. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 903–908. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.06.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clerkin, E. M., & Teachman, B. A. (2010). Training implicit social anxiety associations: An experimental intervention. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 300–308. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.01.001.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clerkin, E. M., & Teachman, B. A. (2011). Training interpretation biases among individuals with symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 337–343. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.01.003.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coles, M. E., Mennin, D. S., & Heimberg, R. G. (2001). Distinguishing obsessive features and worries: The role of thought-action fusion. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 947–959. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00072-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N., & Baker, A. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 5–27. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craske, M. G., & Mystkowski, J. L. (2006). Exposure therapy and extinction: Clinical studies. In M. G. Craske, D. Hermans, & D. Vansteenwegen (Eds.), Fear and learning: From basic processes to clinical implications (pp. 217–233). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Culver, N. C., Stoyanova, M., & Craske, M. G. (2012). Emotional variability and sustained arousal during exposure. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 787–793. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.10.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cuthbert, B. N., Lang, P. J., Strauss, C., Drobes, D., Patrick, C. J., & Bradley, M. M. (2003). The psychophysiology of anxiety disorder: Fear memory imagery. Psychophysiology, 40, 407–422. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.00043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emmelkamp, P. M., van Oppen, P., & Van Balkom, A. J. L. M. (2002). Cognitive changes in patients with obsessive compulsive rituals treated with exposure in vivo and response prevention. In R. O. Frost & G. Steketee (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to obsessions and compulsions: Theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 391–401). Oxford: Elsevier Science.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Figner, B., & Murphy, R. O. (2011). Using skin conductance in judgment and decision making research. In M. Schulte-Mecklenbeck, A. Kuehberger, & R. Ranyard (Eds.), A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., & Cahill, S. P. (2006). Emotional processing theory: An update. In B. O. Rothbaum (Ed.), Pathological anxiety: Emotional processing in etiology and treatment (pp. 3–24). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., Huppert, J. D., Leiberg, S., Langner, R., Kichic, R., Hajcak, G., et al. (2002). The obsessive-compulsive inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychological Assessment, 14, 485–496. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20–35. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hajcak, G., Huppert, J. D., Simons, R. F., & Foa, E. B. (2004). Psychometric properties of the OCI-R in a college sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(1), 115–123. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hallion, L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 940–958. doi:10.1037/a0024355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P., & Mathews, A. (2011). Cognitive bias modification: Past perspectives, current findings, and future applications. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 521–536. doi:10.1177/1745691611421205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, S. G. (2008). Cognitive processes during fear acquisition and extinction in animals and humans: Implications for exposure therapy of anxiety disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 199–210. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.04.009.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, E. A., Lang, T. J., & Shah, D. M. (2009). Developing interpretation bias modification as a “cognitive vaccine” for depressed mood: Imagining positive events makes you feel better than thinking about them verbally. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(1), 76–88. doi:10.1037/a0012590.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Levin, D. N., Miller, G. A., & Kozak, M. J. (1983). Fear behavior, fear imagery, and the psychophysiology of emotion: The problem of affective response integration. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92(3), 276–306. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.92.3.276.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maas, C. J., & Hox, J. J. (2005). Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel modeling. Methodology, 1(3), 86–92. doi:10.1027/1614-1881.1.3.86.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (2012). Cognitive bias modification approaches to anxiety. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 189–217. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magee, J. C., & Teachman, B. A. (2007). Why did the white bear return? Obsessive–compulsive symptoms and attributions for unsuccessful thought suppression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(12), 2884–2898.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (2000). Induced emotional interpretation bias and anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 602–615. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.109.4.602.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167–195. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143916.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., Ridgeway, V., Cook, E., & Yiend, J. (2007). Inducing a benign interpretational bias reduces trait anxiety. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 225–236. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McArdle, J. J., Johnson, R. C., Hishinuma, E. S., Miyamoto, R. H., & Andrade, N. N. (2001). Structural equation modeling of group differences in CES-D ratings of native Hawaiian and non-Hawaiian high school students. Journal of Adolescent Research, 16, 108–149. doi:10.1177/0743558401162002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metzger, R. L. (1976). A reliability and validity study of the state-trait anxiety inventory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32(2), 276–278. doi:10.1002/1097-4679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muris, P., Huijding, J., Mayer, B., Remmerswaal, D., & Vreden, S. (2009). Ground control to major tom: Experimental manipulation of anxiety-related interpretation bias by means of the “space odyssey” paradigm and effects on avoidance tendencies in children. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(3), 333–340. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, R., Hirsch, C., Mathews, A., Smith, K., & Clark, D. M. (2007). Facilitating a benign interpretation bias in a high socially anxious population. Behavior Research and Therapy, 45, 1517–1529. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Najmi, S., & Amir, N. (2010). The effect of attention training on a behavioral test of contamination fears in individuals with subclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119, 136–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narayana, K. V. L., & Bhujanga Rao, D. R. A. (2011). Noise removal using adaptive noise canceling, analysis of ECG using Matlab. International Journal of Engineering Science and Technology, 3, 2839–2844.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (2003). Psychometric validation of the obsessive beliefs questionnaire and the interpretation of intrusions inventory: part I. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 863–878. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00099-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (2005). Psychometric validation of the obsessive belief questionnaire and interpretation of intrusions inventory—Part 2: Factor analyses and testing of a brief version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1527–1542. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.07.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. J. (1998). Empirically supported treatments for children with phobic and anxiety disorders: Current status. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27(2), 156–167. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2702_3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1983). Obstacles to the successful treatment of obsessions. In E. B. Foa & P. Emmelkamp (Eds.), Failures in behavior therapy (pp. 35–57). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1993). Obsessions, responsibility, and guilt. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 149–154. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(93)90066-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 793–802. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00040-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S. (1998). A cognitive theory of obsessions: Elaborations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 385–401.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S., Shafran, R., Mitchell, D., Trant, J., & Teachman, B. (1996). How to remain neutral: An experimental analysis of neutralization. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 889–898. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00051-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. K., & Craske, M. G. (1997). Effects of an expanding-spaced vs massed exposure schedule on fear reduction and return of fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 701–717. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10016-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioral analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23, 571–584. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salkovskis, P. M. (1989). Cognitive-behavioural factors and the persistence of intrusive thoughts in obsessional problems. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 27, 677–692. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(89)90152-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sookman, D., & Pinard, G. (1999). Integrative cognitive therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: A focus on multiple schemas. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 6, 351–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (Form Y). Palo Alto, CA: Mind Garden.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinman, S. A., & Teachman, B. A. (2010). Modifying interpretations among individuals high in anxiety sensitivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 71–78. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.08.008.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sulkowski, M. L., Storch, E. A., Geffken, G. R., Ricketts, E., Murphy, T. K., & Goodman, W. K. (2008). Concurrent validity of the yale-brown obsessive-compulsive scale-symptom checklist. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64, 1338–1351. doi:10.1002/jclp.20525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teachman, B. A., & Addison, L. M. (2008). Training non-threatening interpretations in spider fear. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 448–459. doi:10.1007/s10608-006-9084-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teachman, B. A., Beadel, J. R., & Steinman, S. A. (in press). Mechanisms of change in CBT treatment of anxiety disorders. In P. M. G. Emmelkamp and T. Ehlring (Eds.), International Handbook of Anxiety Disorders: Theory, Research, and Practice, Volume II. Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Tolin, D. E., Woods, C. M., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2003). Relationship between obsessive beliefs and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 657–669. doi:10.1023/A:1026351711837.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venables, P. H., & Christie, M. J. (1980). The effects of age, sex, and time of testing on skin conductance activity. In I. Martin & P. H. Venables (Eds.), Techniques in Psychophysiology (pp. 3–67). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittal, M. L., Woody, S. R., McLean, P. D., Rachman, S. J., & Robichaud, M. (2010). Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(4), 295–303. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, S., Steketee, G., Fama, J., Buhlmann, U., Teachman, B., & Golan, E. (2009). Modular cognitive therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: A waitlist-controlled trial. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 294–305. doi:10.1891/0889-8391.23.4.294.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, S., Steketee, G., Reilly-Harrington, N. A., Deckersbach, T., Buhlmann, U., & Baer, L. (2005). Effectiveness of cognitive therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: An open trial. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 19, 173–179. doi:10.1891/jcop.19.2.173.66792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors are thankful for the feedback provided by members of the Teachman Program for Anxiety, Cognition and Treatment lab. Writing of this article was facilitated by an AG033033 Grant from the National Institute of Health and a Programmatic Distinguished Visitor award from the University of Western Australia awarded to Bethany Teachman.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jessica R. Beadel.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material (DOCX 17 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Beadel, J.R., Smyth, F.L. & Teachman, B.A. Change Processes During Cognitive Bias Modification for Obsessive Compulsive Beliefs. Cogn Ther Res 38, 103–119 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9576-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9576-6

Keywords

Navigation