Skip to main content
Log in

Emotional Interference in Working Memory is Related to Rumination

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Impairments in the executive control function of working memory may underlie moodcongruent attentional and memory biases in depression. Given that the capacity to control and manipulate information held in working memory has been implicated in depressive symptoms and rumination, the present study investigated shifting capacity between internal emotional and nonemotional information in working memory. For this purpose, the Internal Shift Task (IST) was administered to a sample of dysphoric (N = 20) and non-dysphoric undergraduates (N = 20). The main finding was that depressive symptoms in general were not related to impairments in shifting. Interestingly, rumination was related to internal shifting impairments that were most pronounced when negative information was held in working memory. These findings concerning the relation between rumination and shifting impairments are discussed in relation to cognitive vulnerability for depression.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., Hogan, M. E., Witehouse, W. G., Rose, D. T., Robinson, M. S., et al. (2000). The temple-Wisconsin cognitive vulnerability to depression project: Lifetime history of Axis I psychopathology in individuals at high and low cognitive risk for depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 403–418.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Amado-Boccara, I., Gougoulis, N., Poirier, L. M., Galinowski, A., & Loo, H. (1995). Effects of antidepressants on cognitive functions: A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 19, 479–493.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baert, S., De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2011). Modification of information-processing biases in emotional disorders: Clinically relevant developments in experimental psychopathology. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy (in press).

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory (2 nd ed.). The Psychological Association: San Antionio, TX.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychological Review, 8, 77–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernblum, R., & Mor, N. (2010). Rumination and emotion-related biases in refreshing information. Emotion, 10(3), 423–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 303–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific foundations of cognitive theory and therapy for depression. John Wiley & Sons: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daches, S., Mor, N., Winquist, J., & Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2010). Brooding and attentional control in processing self-encoded information: Evidence from a modified Garner Task. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 876–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 699–711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2010). The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 264–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Understanding increasing vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: A reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual model. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 50–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deveney, C. M., & Deldin, P. J. (2006). A preliminary investigation of cognitive flexibility for emotional information in major depressive disorders and non-psychiatric controls. Emotion, 6, 429–437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., Ridder, E. M., & Beautrais, A. L. (2005). Subthreshold depression in adolescence and mental health outcomes in adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 66–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garavan, H. (1998). Serial attention within working memory. Memory & Cognition, 26, 263–276.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Ben-Artzi, E., Jeczemien, P., Marom, S., & Hermesh, H. (2004). Depression impairs the ability to ignore the emotional aspects of facial expressions: Evidence from the Garner task. Cognition & Emotion, 18(2), 209–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Baert, S., & Koster, E. H. W. (2006). Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 93, 149–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goeleven, E., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & Verschuere, B. (2008). The Karolinska directed emotional faces: A validation study. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 1094–1118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Hammen, C. I. (2002). Handbook of Depression. Guilford Press: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P. O., Le Bastard, G., Pochon, J. B., Levy, R., Allilaire, J. F., Dubois, B., et al. (2004). Executive functions and updating of the contents of working memory in unipolar depressions. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 38, 567–576.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hertel, P. T. (2007). Impairments in inhibition or cognitive control in psychological disorders. Applied & Preventive Psychology, 12(3), 149–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J., Lewis, R. L., Nee, D. E., Lusting, C. A., Berman, M. G., & Moore, K. S. (2007). The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 193–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J. (2004). Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes. Cognition & Emotion, 18, 125–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J. (2006). The relation of rumination and inhibition: Evidence from a negative priming task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 149–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy, 37, 269–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 80–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joormann, J., Yoon, K. L., & Zetsche, U. (2007). Cognitive inhibition in depression. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12, 128–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., Merikangaz, K. R., et al. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 3095–3105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review.

  • Koster, E. H. W., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and recall bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 219–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces (KDEF). Karolinska Hospital, Stockholm: Department of Neurosciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merriam, E. P., Thase, M. E., Haas, G. L., Keshavan, M. S., & Sweeney, J. A. (1999). Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in depression determined by Wisconsin card sorting performance. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 780–782.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (1996). Control of mental processes. In V. Bruce (Ed.), Unsolved mysteries of the mind: Tutorial essays in cognition (pp. 93–148). Hove, UK: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Consiousness and Cognition, 18, 176–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, T., Brewin, C. R., Wheatley, J., Wells, A., Fisher, P., & Myers, S. (2007). Intrusive images and memories in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2573–2580.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raes, F., & Hermans, D. (2007). The revised version of the Dutch Ruminative Response Scale. Unpublished instrument.

  • Raes, F., Schoofs, H., Hoes, D., Hermans, D., Van Den Eede, F., & Franck, E. (2009). ‘Reflection’ en ‘brooding’ als subtypes van rumineren: een herziening van de Ruminative Response Scale. [‘Reflection’ and ‘brooding’ as two subtypes of rumination: A revision of the Ruminative Response Scale]. Gedragstherapie, 42, 205–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J., Ghinassi, F., & Thase, M. E. (2007). Neurobehavioral therapies in the 21st century: Summary of an emerging field and an extended example of cognitive control training for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 262–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, D., Keller, M., Leon, A., Mueller, T., Lavori, P., Shea, M., et al. (2000). Multiple recurrences of major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 229–233.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Does, A. J. W. (2002). Handleiding: De Nederlandse versie van de Beck Depression Inventory, 2de Editie (The Dutch version of the Beck Depression Inventory, 2 nd Edition). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger b.v.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 72, 400–402.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Whitmer, A. J., & Banich, M. T. (2007). Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychological Science, 18, 546–553.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Evi De Lissnyder.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E.H.W. & De Raedt, R. Emotional Interference in Working Memory is Related to Rumination. Cogn Ther Res 36, 348–357 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9352-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9352-4

Keywords

Navigation