Skip to main content
Log in

A Comparison of Problem-Solving and Spatial Orientation in the Wolf (Canis lupus) and Dog (Canis familiaris)

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study I tested Benson Ginsburg’s theory that dogs should show diminished ability, compared to wolves, in orienting in three-dimensional space and manipulating objects sequentially. Dogs of all ages and juvenile wolves should do poorly on these measures, but at some time before sexual maturity, the juvenile wolves should begin improving to the level of adult wolves. Two adult and seven juvenile wolves were compared with 40 adult German shepherds. The initial task was to pull a single rope suspended from the ceiling; complexity was increased by the addition of ropes and by changing spatial configurations. Adult wolf performance was consistently successful across all tests and requirements. Juvenile wolves had little difficulty with one and two rope tests, but did more poorly in the three rope tests. The behavior of the dogs grouped into four profiles (# of dogs): non-responders (6), one rope (15), two rope (14), and three rope responders (5).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen DL (1979) Wolves of minong. Houghton Mifflin, Boston, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakarich AC (1979) Comparative development in wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). MS thesis, Trinity University, San Antonio, Texas

  • Bayer K, Range F, Aust U, Steurer M, Huber L (2009) The touch-screen method as an implement for dog experiments. J Vet Behav: Clin Appl Res 4:51 (Abstract 12)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff M (1974) Social play in coyotes, wolves, and dogs. Bioscience 24:225–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brower K (1989) Paul Paquet: wolf man of riding mountain. Wolves Relat Canids 2:15–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Buytendijk FJJ (1936) The mind of the dog. Houghton Mifflin, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Carbyn LN, Trottier T (1988) Descriptions of wolf attacks on bison calves in Wood Buffalo National Park [Canada]. Arctic 41:297–302

    Google Scholar 

  • Coppinger L, Coppinger R (1982) Livestock-guarding dogs that wear sheep’s clothing. Smithsonian 13:64–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Coppinger R, Glendinning J, Torop E, Matthay C, Sutherland M, Smith C (1987) Degree of behavioral neoteny differentiates canid polymorphs. Ethology 75:89–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crisler L (1958) Arctic wild. Harper and Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon JS (1934) Mother wolf carries food twelve miles to her young. J Mammal 15:158

    Google Scholar 

  • Enloe LJ (1988) Problem solving and delayed response performance of domestic dogs (C. familiaris) and hand-reared coyotes (C. latrans). Unpublished manuscript, Idaho State University

  • Fentress JC (1967) Observations on the behavioral development of a hand-reared male timber wolf. Am Zool 7:339–351

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox MW (1971) Behaviour of wolves, dogs and related canids. Harper and Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox MW (1978) Man, wolf, and dog. In: Hall RL, Sharp HS (eds) Wolf and man. Academic, New York, pp 19–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank H (1980) Evolution of canine information processing under conditions of natural and artificial selection. Z Tierpsychol 53:389–399

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frank H, Frank MG (1982) Comparison of problem-solving performance in six-week-old wolves and dogs. Anim Behav 30:95–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank H, Frank MG (1985) Comparative manipulation-test performance in ten-week-old wolves (Canis lupus) and Alaskan Malamutes (Canis familiaris): a Piagetian interpretation. J Comp Psychol 99:266–274

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank H, Frank MG (1987) The University of Michigan canine information-processing project (1979–1981). In: Frank H (ed) Man and wolf. Dr. W. Junk, Dordrecht, The Hague

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank H, Hasselbach LM, Littleton DM (1986) Socialized vs. unsocialized wolves (Canis lupus) in experimental research. In: Fox MW, Mickley LD (eds) Advances in animal welfare science 1986/87. The Humane Society of the United States, Washington, DC, pp 33–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller JL, Fox MW (1969) The behavior of dogs. In: Hafez ESE (ed) The behavior of domestic animals, 2nd edn. Williams and Wilkins, Baltimore, MD, pp 438–481

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg BE (1966) Social behavior and social hierarchy in the formation of personality profiles in animals. In: Zubin J (ed) Comparative psychopathology, animal and human. Proceedings of the American Psychopathological Association, vol 22. Grune and Stratton, New York, pp 95–114

  • Ginsburg BE (1978) The genetics of social behavior. In: Bateson PPG, Klopfer PH (eds) Perspectives in ethology, 3rd edn. Plenum, New York, pp 1–15

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg BE, Hiestand L (1992) Humanity’s “best friend”: the origins of our inevitable bond with dogs. In: Davis H, Balfour D (eds) The inevitable bond. Cambridge University, New York, pp 93–108

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg BE, Schotté CS (1978) Genetic differences determining the capacities for social behavior among wolves and dogs. Behav Genet 8:94 (Abstract)

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg BE, Slatis H (1962) The use of purebred dogs in research: problems of genetics. Proc Anim Care Panel 12:151–156

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gould SJ (1986) The egg-a-day barrier. Nat Hist 95:16–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiestand NL (1989) A comparison of problem-solving and spatial orientation in the wolf (Canis lupus) and dog (Canis familiaris). PhD dissertation, Department Biobehavioral Sciences, The University of Connecticut

  • Hill BT (1981) Data base organization for behavioral research. Unpublished manuscript, Dept Biobehavioral Sciences, The University of Connecticut

  • Humphrey E, Warner L (1934) Working dogs. Johns Hopkins, Baltimore, MD

    Google Scholar 

  • Iljin NA (1941) Wolf-dog genetics. J Genet 42:359–414

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenks SM (1988) Behavioral regulation of social organization and mating in a captive wolf pack. PhD dissertation, Department Biobehavioral Sciences, The University of Connecticut

  • Kleiman DG (1967) Some aspects of social behavior in the Canidae. Am Zool 7:365–372

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley KS, Wade M (1946) The Pavlovian theory of generalization. Psychol Rev 53:72–87

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mech LD (1966) Hunting behavior of timber wolves in Minnesota. J Mammal 47:347–348

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mech LD (1970) The wolf: the ecology and behavior of an endangered species. Natural History, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mech LD, Korb M (1978) An unusually long pursuit of a deer by a wolf. J Mammal 59:860–861

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Medjo DC, Mech LD (1976) Reproductive activity in nine- and ten-month-old wolves. J Mammal 57:406–408

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morell V (2009) Going to the dogs. Science 325:1062–1065

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Möslinger H, Kotrschal K, Huber L, Range F, Virányi Z (2009) Cooperative string-pulling in wolves. J Vet Behav: Clin Appl Res 4:99 Abstract 93

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Packard JM, Seal US, Mech LD, Plotka ED (1985) Causes of reproductive failure in two family groups of wolves (Canis lupus). Z Tierpsychol 68:24–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters R (1978) Communication, cognitive mapping, and strategy in wolves and hominids. In: Hall RL, Sharp HS (eds) Wolf and man. Academic, New York, pp 95–107

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters R, Mech LD (1975) Behavioral and intellectual adaptations of selected mammalian predators to the problem of hunting large animals. In: Tuttle RH (ed) Socioecology and the psychology of primates. Mouton, The Hague, pp 279–300

    Google Scholar 

  • Price EO (1984) Behavioral aspects of animal domestication. Q Rev Biol 59:1–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roitblat HL (1982) The meaning of representation in animal memory. Behav Brain Sci 5:353–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter RJ, Pimlott DH (1968) The world of the wolf. J. B. Lippincott, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Savalli C, Brandão MM, Domingues TS, Honório MA, Ades C (2009) Dog-human communication through a keyboard: is human attentional state relevant? J Vet Behav: Clin Appl Res 4:55 Abstract 19

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schenkel R (1947) Expression studies of wolves. Behaviour 1:81–129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schotté CS (1988) The development of social organization and mating patterns in a captive wolf (Canis lupus) pack. PhD dissertation, Department of Biobehavioral Sciences, The University of Connecticut

  • Scott JP (1950) The social behavior of dogs and wolves: an illustration of sociobiological systematics. Ann New York Acad Sci 51:1009–1021

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott JP (1967) The evolution of social behavior in dogs and wolves. Am Zool 7:373–381

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott JP, Fuller JL (1965) Dog behavior: the genetic basis. University of Chicago, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Slade VR (1983) Individual differences in social bond formation of two hand-reared prepubertal wolves. MS thesis, The University of Connecticut

  • Stockard CR, Anderson OD, James WT (1941) The genetic and endocrinic basis for differences in form and behavior. Wistar Institute, Philadelphia, PA

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland NS, Mackintosh NJ (1971) Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. Academic, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweeney JR, Marchinton RL, Sweeney JM (1971) Responses of radio-monitored white-tailed deer chased by hunting dogs. J Wildlife Manage 35:707–716

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike EL (1911) Animal intelligence. Hafner, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Warden CJ, Warner LH (1928) The sensory capacities and intelligence of dogs, with a report on the ability of the noted dog “Fellow” to respond to verbal stimuli. Q Rev Biol 3:1–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodbury CB (1943) The learning of stimulus patterns by dogs. J Comp Physiol Psychol 58:317–320

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolpy JH (1968) Socialization of wolves. In: Masserman JH (ed) Animal and human. Grune and Stratton, New York, pp 82–94

    Google Scholar 

  • Young SP, Goldman EA (1944) The wolves of North America, part I. Dover, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimen E (1971) Wölfe und Königspudel. R. Piper & Co, Munich

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimen E (1981) The wolf: a species in danger. Delacorte, New York

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laurie Hiestand.

Additional information

Edited by Stephen Maxson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hiestand, L. A Comparison of Problem-Solving and Spatial Orientation in the Wolf (Canis lupus) and Dog (Canis familiaris). Behav Genet 41, 840–857 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-011-9455-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-011-9455-4

Keywords

Navigation