Skip to main content
Log in

Spatial Ability and Prenatal Androgens: Meta-Analyses of Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia and Digit Ratio (2D:4D) Studies

  • ORIGINAL PAPER: MINOT SPECIAL ISSUE
  • Published:
Archives of Sexual Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Hormonal manipulations indicate that early androgens organize sex differences in spatial ability in laboratory rats. In humans, spatial ability is also sexually dimorphic, and information about the effects of prenatal androgens on spatial ability can be obtained from studies of congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH) and the ratio of the second and fourth finger lengths (2D:4D). CAH is characterized by prenatal overproduction of adrenal androgens and several lines of evidence suggest that 2D:4D reflects prenatal androgen exposure. Some studies have found that these proxy measures of prenatal androgens predict spatial ability, others have found no significant relationship, and yet others have obtained results in the opposite direction. In light of these mixed findings, we conducted meta-analyses of published literature and unpublished results to determine if, across studies, either of these indicators of prenatal androgens predicts performance on spatial tasks that show a male advantage. In addition, we applied a trim and fill analysis to the data in search of asymmetry that might be an indication of publication bias. Results indicated that females with CAH perform better on these spatial tasks, and CAH males perform worse, than do controls. Little or no relationship exists between 2D:4D and spatial ability. Implications for possible hormonal contributions and the developmental timing of sex differences in spatial cognition are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Astur, R. S., Ortiz, M. L., & Sutherland, R. J. (1998). A characterization of performance by men and women in a virtual Morris water task: A large and reliable sex difference. Behavioural Brain Research, 93, 185–190.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, E. J., Manning, J. T., McInroy, K., & Mathews, E. (2002). A preliminary investigation of the associations between personality, cognitive ability and digit ratio. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1115–1124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, S. W., & Ehrhardt, A. A. (1974). Prenatal androgen, intelligence, and cognitive sex differences. In R. C. Friedman, R. M. Richart, & R. L. Vande Wiele (Eds.), Sex differences in behavior (pp. 53–76). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A. (1999). Effects of early androgens on sex-typed activities and interests in adolescents with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 35, 102–110.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A. (2001). Cognitive function in congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Endocrinology and Metabolism Clinics of North America, 30, 173–192.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bodner, G. M., & Guay, R. B. (1997). The Purdue visualization of rotations test. The Chemical Educator, 2, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis Version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowman, R. E., MacLusky, N. J., Sarmiento, Y., Frankfurt, M., Gordon, M., & Luine, V. N. (2004). Sexually dimorphic effects of prenatal stress on cognition, hormonal responses, and central neurotransmitters. Endocrinology, 145, 3778–3787.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, W. M., Hines, M., Fane, B. A., & Breedlove, S. M. (2002). Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior, 42, 380–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buck, J. J., Williams, R. M., Hughes, I. A., & Acerini, C. L. (2003). In-utero androgen exposure and 2nd to 4th digit length ratio-comparisons between healthy control and females with classical congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Human Reproduction, 18, 976–979.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole-Harding, S., Morstad, A. L., & Wilson, J. R. (1988). Spatial ability in members of opposite-sex twin pairs. Behavior Genetics, 18, 710.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolican, J., & Peters, M. (2003). Sexual dimorphism in the 2D/4D ratio and its relation to mental rotation performance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 179–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1973). Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 75–176). Oxford: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csatho, A., Osvath, A., Karadi, K., Bicsak, E., Manning, J. T., & Kallai, J. (2003). Spatial navigation related to the ratio of second to fourth digit length in women. Learning and Individual Differences, 13, 239–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, J. L., Cheung, Y. M., & Lau, R. T. (1975). Developmental effects of neonatal sex hormones on spatial and activity skills in the white rat. Biological Psychology, 3, 213–229.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dorr, H. G., & Sippell, W. G. (1993). Prenatal dexamethasone treatment in pregnancies at risk for congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency: Effect on midgestational amniotic fluid steroid levels. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 76, 117–120.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Driscoll, I., Hamilton, D. A., Petropoulos, H., Yeo, R. A., Brooks, W. M., Baumgartner, R. N., et al. (2003). The aging hippocampus: Cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 1344–1351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driscoll, I., Hamilton, D. A., Yeo, R. A., Brooks, W. M., & Sutherland, R. J. (2005). Virtual navigation in humans: The impact of age, sex, and hormones on place learning. Hormones and Behavior, 47, 326–335.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dureman, I., Kebbon, L., & Osterberg, E. (1971). Manual till DS-batteriet. Stockholm: Psykologiforlaget.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duval, S. J., & Tweedie, R. L. (2000). A non-parametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 95, 89–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, D. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimshaw, G. M., Sitarenios, G., & Finegan, J. A. (1995). Mental rotation at 7 years: Relations with prenatal testosterone levels and spatial play experiences. Brain and Cognition, 29, 85–100.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hampson, E., Rovet, J. F., & Altmann, D. (1998). Spatial reasoning in children with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Developmental Neuropsychology, 14, 299–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Healy, W. (1914). A pictorial completion test. Psychological Review, 21, 189–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helleday, J., Bartfai, A., Ritzen, E. M., & Forsman, M. (1994). General intelligence and cognitive profile in women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH). Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19, 343–356.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M. (2004). Brain gender. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M., Fane, B. A., Pasterski, V. L., Mathews, G. A., Conway, G. S., & Brook, C. (2003). Spatial abilities following prenatal androgen abnormality: Targeting and mental rotations performance in individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28, 1010–1026.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hojbjerg Gravholt, C., Svenstrup, B., Bennett, P., & Sandahl Christiansen, J. (1999). Reduced androgen levels in adult turner syndrome: Influence of female sex steroids and growth hormone status. Clinical Endocrinology, 50, 791–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imperato-McGinley, J., Peterson, R. E., Gautier, T., Cooper, G., Danner, R., Arthur, A., et al. (1982). Hormonal evaluation of a large kindred with complete androgen insensitivity: Evidence for secondary 5 alpha-reductase deficiency. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 54, 931–941.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Imperato-McGinley, J., Pichardo, M., Gautier, T., Voyer, D., & Bryden, M. P. (1991). Cognitive abilities in androgen-insensitive subjects: Comparison with control males and females from the same kindred. Clinical Endocrinology, 34, 341–347.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Isgor, C., & Sengelaub, D. R. (1998). Prenatal gonadal steroids affect adult spatial behavior, CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cell morphology in rats. Hormones and Behavior, 34, 183–198.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Isgor, C., & Sengelaub, D. R. (2003). Effects of neonatal gonadal steroids on adult CA3 pyramidal neuron dendritic morphology and spatial memory in rats. Journal of Neurobiology, 55, 179–190.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, D. N. I., Vernon, P. A., & Jackson, D. N. (1993). Dynamic spatial performance and general intelligence. Intelligence, 17, 451–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jager, A. O., & Althoff, K. (1983). WILDE-intelligenz-test. Gottingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonasson, Z. (2005). Meta-analysis of sex differences in rodent models of learning and memory: A review of behavioral and biological data. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 811–825.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, R., Hess, S., & Birecree, E. (1978). Effects of hormone manipulations and exploration on sex differences in maze learning. Behavioral Biology, 24, 364–377.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kempel, P., Gohlke, B., Klempau, J., Zinsberger, P., Reuter, M., & Hennig, J. (2005). Second-to-fourth digit lengths, testosterone and spatial ability. Intelligence, 33, 215–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitraki, E., Kremmyda, O., Youlatos, D., Alexis, M. N., & Kittas, C. (2004). Gender-dependent alterations in corticosteroid receptor status and spatial performance following 21 days of restraint stress. Neuroscience, 125, 47–55.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malas, M. A., Dogan, S., Hilal Evcil, E., & Desdicioglu, K. (2006). Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio (2D:4D). Early Human Development, 82, 469–475.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malouf, M. A., Migeon, C. J., Carson, K. A., Petrucci, L., & Wisniewski, A. B. (2006). Cognitive outcome in adult women affected by congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Hormone Research, 65, 142–150.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V. A., Sasanuma, S., Sakuma, N., & Masaki, S. (1990). Sex differences in cognitive abilities: A cross-cultural perspective. Neuropsychologia, 28, 1063–1077.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T. (2002). Digit ratio: A pointer to fertility, behavior, and health. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., Bundred, P. E., Newton, D. J., & Flanagan, B. F. (2003). The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 399–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., Scutt, D., Wilson, J., & Lewis-Jones, D. I. (1998). The ratio of the 2nd and 4th digit length: A predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen. Human Reproduction, 13, 3000–3004.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, J. T., & Taylor, R. P. (2001). Second to fourth digit ratio and male ability in sport: Implications for sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 61–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayes, J. T., & Jahoda, G. (1988). Patterns of visual-spatial performance and ‘spatial ability’: Dissociation of ethnic and sex differences. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 105–119.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McBurney, D. H., Gaulin, S. J. C., Devineni, T., & Adams, C. (1997). Superior spatial ability of women: Stronger evidence for the gathering hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 167–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, D., Loehlin, J. C., Breedlove, S. M., Lippa, R. A., Manning, J. T., & Rahman, Q. (2005). A reanalysis of five studies on sexual orientation and the relative length of the 2nd and 4th fingers (the 2D:4D ratio). Archives of Sexual Behavior, 34, 341–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFadden, D., & Schubel, E. (2003). The relationships between otoacoustic emissions and relative lengths of fingers and toes in humans. Hormones and Behavior, 43, 421–429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, L. S., Ryan, K. O., & Omenn, G. S. (1975). Congenital adrenal hyperplasia. II. Cognitive and behavioral studies. Behavior Genetics, 5, 175–188.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, R. G. M. (1981). Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learning and Motivation, 12, 239–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M. W., Eling, P. A., & Otten, B. J. (2003). A review of neuropsychological and motor studies in Turner Syndrome. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 329–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunez, J. L., & McCarthy, M. M. (2003). Estradiol exacerbates hippocampal damage in a model of preterm infant brain injury. Endocrinology, 144, 2350–2359.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Okten, A., Kalyoncu, M., & Yaris, N. (2002). The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Early Human Development, 70, 47–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Shaughnessy, P. J., Baker, P. J., & Johnston, H. (2006). The foetal Leydig cell—Differentiation, function and regulation. International Journal of Andrology, 29, 90–95.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, K., & Lynn, R. (1993). Sex differences in primary cognitive abilities among blacks, Indians and whites in South Africa. Journal of Biosocial Science, 25, 557–560.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pang, S., Levine, L. S., Cederqvist, L. L., Fuentes, M., Riccardi, V. M., Holcombe, J. H., et al. (1980). Amniotic fluid concentrations of delta 5 and delta 4 steroids in fetuses with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21 hydroxylase deficiency and in anencephalic fetuses. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 51, 223–229.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Perlman, S. M. (1973). Cognitive abilities of children with hormone abnormalities: Screening by psychoeducational tests. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 6, 26–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, M., Manning, J. T., & Reimers, S. (2007). The effects of sex, sexual orientation, and digit ratio (2D:4D) on mental rotation performance. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36, 251–260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin, M., O’Connell, R. L., & Freeman, L. M. (2004). Picture recall skills correlate with 2D:4D ratio in women but not in men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 174–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J., & Breedlove, S. M. (2007). Sex differences in spatial ability: Evolution, hormones and the brain. In S. M. Platek, J. P. Keenan, & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Evolutionary cognitive neuroscience (pp. 329–379). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putz, D. A., Gaulin, S. J., Sporter, R. J., & McBurney, D. H. (2004). Sex hormones and finger length: What does 2D:4D indicate? Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 182–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rahman, Q., Wilson, G. D., & Abrahams, S. (2004). Biosocial factors, sexual orientation and neurocognitive functioning. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 867–881.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, S. M., Berenbaum, S. A., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. (1986). Early hormonal influences on cognitive functioning in congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Developmental Psychology, 22, 191–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ripa, C. P. L., Johannsen, T. H., Mortensen, E. L., & Muller, J. (2003). General cognitive functions, mental rotations ability, and handedness in adult females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia [Abstract]. Hormones and Behavior, 44, 72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roof, R. L. (1993). Neonatal exogenous testosterone modifies sex difference in radial arm and Morris water maze performance in prepubescent and adult rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 53, 1–10.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Roof, R. L., & Havens, M. D. (1992). Testosterone improves maze performance and induces development of a male hippocampus in females. Brain Research, 572, 310–313.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, J. L., Stefanatos, G. A., Kushner, H., Zinn, A., Bondy, C., & Roeltgen, D. (2002). Persistent cognitive deficits in adult women with Turner syndrome. Neurology, 58, 218–225.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rothstein, H., Sutton, A. J., & Borenstein, M. (2005). Publication bias in meta analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments. Chichester, UK: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, G., Bereckzei, T., Csatho, A., & Manning, J. T. (2005). The ratio of the 2nd to 4th finger length predicts spatial ability in men but not women. Cortex, 41, 789–795.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scarbrough, P. S., & Johnston, V. S. (2005). Individual differences in women’s facial preferences as a function of digit ratio and mental rotation ability. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 509–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, I., & Eals, M. (1992). Sex differences in spatial abilities: Evolutionary theory and data. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 533–549). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, J., Skvarenina, A., & Pottier, J. (1975). Effects of neonatal androgens on open-field behavior and maze learning in the prepubescent and adult rat. Physiology and Behavior, 14, 291–295.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L. L., & Thurstone, T. G. (1963). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: Science Research Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Anders, S. M., & Hampson, E. (2005). Testing the prenatal androgen hypothesis: Measuring digit ratios, sexual orientation, and spatial abilities in adults. Hormones and Behavior, 47, 92–98.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations: A group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599–604.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vicedomini, J. P., Nonneman, A. J., DeKosky, S. T., & Scheff, S. W. (1986). Perinatal glucocorticoids disrupt learning: A sexually dimorphic response. Physiology and Behavior, 36, 145–149.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C. L., Barnett, A. M., & Meck, W. H. (1990). Organizational effects of early gonadal secretions on sexual differentiation in spatial memory. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 84–97.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, T. J., Pepitone, M. E., Christensen, S. E., Cooke, B. M., Huberman, A. D., Breedlove, N. J., et al. (2000). Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation. Nature, 404, 455–456.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wudy, S. A., Dorr, H. G., Solleder, C., Djalali, M., & Homoki, J. (1999). Profiling steroid hormones in amniotic fluid of midpregnancy by routine stable isotope dilution/gas chromatography-mass spectrometry: Reference values and concentrations in fetuses at risk for 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 84, 2724–2728.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David A. Puts.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Puts, D.A., McDaniel, M.A., Jordan, C.L. et al. Spatial Ability and Prenatal Androgens: Meta-Analyses of Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia and Digit Ratio (2D:4D) Studies. Arch Sex Behav 37, 100–111 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9271-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9271-3

Keywords

Navigation