Skip to main content
Log in

When Experts Argue: Explaining the Best and the Worst of Reasoning

  • Published:
Argumentation Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Expert reasoning is responsible for some of the most stunning human achievements, but also for some of the most disastrous decisions ever made. The argumentative theory of reasoning has proven very effective at explaining the pattern of reasoning’s successes and failures. In the present article, it is expanded to account for expert reasoning. The argumentative theory predicts that reasoning should display a strong confirmation bias. If argument quality is not sufficiently high in a domain, the confirmation bias will make experts tap into their vast knowledge to defend whatever opinion they hold, with polarization and overconfidence as expected results. By contrast, experts should benefit even more from the power of group discussion to make the best of the confirmation bias—when they genuinely disagree that is, otherwise polarization is again likely to ensue. When experts interact with laymen other mechanisms can take the lead, in particular trust calibration and consistency checking. They can yield poor outcomes if experts do not have a sustained interaction with laymen, or if the laymen have strong opinions when they witness a debate between experts. Seeing reasoning as a mechanism of epistemic vigilance aimed at finding and evaluating arguments helps make better sense of expert reasoning performance, be it in individual ratiocination, in debates with other experts, or in interactions with laymen.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allgeier, A.R., D. Byrne, B. Brooks, and D. Revnes. 1979. The waffle phenomenon: Negative evaluations of those who shift attitudinally. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 9(2): 170–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailenson, J.N., and L.J. Rips. 1996. Informal reasoning and burden of proof. Applied Cognitive Psychology 10(7): 3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bainbridge, S.M. 2002. Why a board? Group decision making in corporate governance. Vanderbilt Law Review 55: 1–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. 1995. Myside bias in thinking about abortion. Thinking and Reasoning 1: 221–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birch, S., and P. Bloom. 2003. Children are cursed: An asymmetric bias in mental-state attribution. Psychological Science 14(3): 283–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bothwell, R.K., and J. Brigham. 1983. Selective evaluation and recall during the 1980 Reagan-carter debate 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 13(5): 427–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cassidy, J. 2010. After the blowup. New Yorker 85(44): 28–33. http://www.newyorker.com/reporting/2010/01/11/100111fa_fact_cassidy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaiken, S., and D. Maheswaran. 1994. Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66(3): 460–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chambers, S. 2004. Behind closed doors: Publicity, secrecy, and the quality of deliberation. Journal of Political Philosophy 12(4): 389–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, S.G., and D.E. Bailey. 1997. What makes teams work: Group effectiveness research from the shop floor to the executive suite. Journal of management 23(3): 239–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, F.B., and E.H. Tiller. 1998. Judicial partisanship and obedience to legal doctrine: Whistleblowing on the federal courts of appeals. Yale Law Journal 107(7): 2155–2176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R., and J.R. Krebs. 1979. Arms races between and within species. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences 205(1161): 489–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Vries, M., C.L. Witteman, R.W. Holland, and A. Dijksterhuis. 2010. The unconscious thought effect in clinical decision making: An Example in diagnosis. Medical Decision Making 30(5): 578–581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denes-Raj, V., and S. Epstein. 1994. Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behave against their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66(5): 819–829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dessalles, J. 2007. Why we talk: The evolutionary origins of language. Cambridge: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dienes, Z., and J. Perner. 1999. A theory of implicit and explicit knowledge. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(05): 735–808.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., M.W. Bos, A. van der Leij, and R.B. van Baaren. 2009. Predicting soccer matches after unconscious and conscious thought as a function of expertise. Psychological Science 20(11): 1381–1387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dubreuil, B. 2010. Paleolithic public goods games: Why human culture and cooperation did not evolve in one step. Biology and Philosophy 25(1): 53–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, K. 1995. How scientists really reason: Scientific reasoning in real-world laboratories. In The nature of insight, ed. R.J. Sternberg and J. Davidson, 365–395. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Elster, J. 1995. Strategic uses of argument. In Barriers to conflict resolution, ed. K. Arrow, R. Mnookin, L. Ross, A. Tversky, and R. Wilson, 236–257. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. 2002. Logic and human reasoning: An assessment of the deduction paradigm. Psychological Bulletin 128(6): 978–996.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. 2007. Hypothetical thinking: Dual processes in reasoning and judgment. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. 1957. A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gladwell, M. 2005. Blink: The power of thinking without thinking. Little, Brown: Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, K., J. Armstrong, and A. Graefe. 2007. Methods to elicit forecasts from groups delphi and prediction markets compared. MPRA paper no. 4663.

  • Hahn, U., and M. Oaksford. 2007. The rationality of informal argumentation: A bayesian approach to reasoning fallacies. Psychological Review 114(3): 704–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halberstam, D. 2001. The best and the brightest. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heesacker, M., R. Petty, and J. Cacioppo. 1983. Field dependence and attitude change: Source credibility can alter persuasion by affecting message-relevant thinking. Journal of Personality 51(4): 653–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinds, P.J. 1999. The curse of expertise: The effects of expertise and debiasing methods on predictions of novice performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 5: 205–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy, S. 2009. Mothers and others. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janis, I.L. 1982. Groupthink, vol. 2. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., S. Lichtenstein, and B. Fischhoff. 1980. Reasons for confidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory and Cognition 6: 107–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J.R., and R. Dawkins. 1984. Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation? In Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, vol. 2, ed. J.R. Krebs and N.B. Davies, 390–402. Oxford: Basil Blackwell Scientific Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. 1991. The Skills of arguments. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., and A. Aktipis. 2007. Modularity and the social mind: Are psychologists too self-ish? Personality and Social Psychology Review 11(2): 131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laughlin, P., and A. Ellis. 1986. Demonstrability and social combination processes on mathematical intellective tasks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 22: 177–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, K. 2004. Dialectical practice in Tibetan philosophical culture: An ethnomethodological inquiry into formal reasoning. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombardelli, C., J. Proudman, and J. Talbot. 2005. Committees versus individuals: An experimental analysis of monetary policy decision-making. International Journal of Central Banking 1(May): 181–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., L. Ross, and M. Lepper. 1979. Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37(11): 2098–2109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mamede, S., H.G. Schmidt, R.M. Rikers, E.J. Custers, T.A. Splinter, and J.L. van Saase. 2010. Conscious thought beats deliberation without attention in diagnostic decision-making: At least when you are an expert. Psychological Research 74(6): 586–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H. In press a. Our pigheaded core: How we became smarter to be influenced by other people. In Evolution, cooperation, and complexity, eds. B. Calcott, R. Joyce, and K. Sterelny. Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Mercier, H. In press b. Reasoning serves argumentation in children. Cognitive Development.

  • Mercier, H. In press c. What good is moral reasoning? Mind & Society.

  • Mercier, H. 2011. On the universality of argumentative reasoning. Journal of Cognition and Culture 11: 85–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H., and B.R. Strickland. Submitted. Evaluating arguments from the reaction of the audience.

  • Mercier, H., and H. Landemore. In press. Reasoning is for arguing: Understanding the successes and failures of deliberation. Political Psychology.

  • Mercier, H., and D. Sperber. 2009. Intuitive and reflective inferences. In In two minds, ed. J. Evans and K. Frankish. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H., and D. Sperber. 2011a. Argumentation: Its adaptiveness and efficacy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34(2): 94–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercier, H., and D. Sperber. 2011b. Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34(2): 57–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millar, M.G., and A. Tesser. 1986. Thought-induced attitude change: The effects of schema structure and commitment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51(2): 259–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moshman, D., and M. Geil. 1998. Collaborative reasoning: Evidence for collective rationality. Thinking and Reasoning 4(3): 231–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munro, G.D., P.H. Ditto, L.K. Lockhart, A. Fagerlin, M. Gready, and E. Peterson. 2002. Biased assimilation of sociopolitical arguments: Evaluating the 1996 US presidential debate. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 24(1): 15–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novaes, C.D. 2005. Medieval Obligationes as logical games of consistency maintenance. Synthese 145(3): 371–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petty, R., J. Cacioppo, and R. Goldman. 1981. Personal involvement as a determinant of argument-based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 41(5): 847–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, E.M., S. Chaiken, and R.S. Tordesillas. 1995. Attitude strength and resistance processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69(3): 408–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L.B., M. Salmon, C.M. Zeitz, S.H. Wathen, and M. Holowchak. 1993. Reasoning in conversation. Cognition and Instruction 11(3/4): 347–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Revesz, R.L. 1997. Environmental Regulation, Ideology, and the DC Circuit. Virginia Law Review 83(8): 1717–1772.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, G., and G. Wright. 1999. The Delphi technique as a forecasting tool: Issues and analysis. International Journal of Forecasting 15(4): 353–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shafir, E., I. Simonson, and A. Tversky. 1993. Reason-based choice. Cognition 49(1–2): 11–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, R. 1995. Cooperative learning: Theory, research, and practice, vol. 2. London: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. 2000. Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective, ed. D. Sperber, 117–137. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. 2001. An evolutionary perspective on testimony and argumentation. Philosophical Topics 29: 401–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D., F. Clément, C. Heintz, O. Mascaro, H. Mercier, G. Origgi, and D. Wilson. 2010. Epistemic vigilance 25(4): 359–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. 2004. The robot’s rebellion. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. In press. The fate of the third chimpanzee. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Taber, C., and M. Lodge. 2006. Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science 50(3): 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tedeschi, J.T., R.B. Schlenker, and T.V. Bonoma. 1971. Cognitive dissonance: Private ratiocination or public spectacle. American Psychologist 26(8): 685–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, A., and M.C. Conlee. 1975. Some effects of time and thought on attitude polarization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31(2): 262–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tesser, A., and C. Leone. 1977. Cognitive schemas and thought as determinants of attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 13(4): 340–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. 2005. Expert political judgment: How good is it? How can we know? Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., M. Carpenter, J. Call, T. Behne, and H. Moll. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5): 675–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., and L. Cosmides. 1992. The psychological foundations of culture. In The adapted mind, ed. J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby, 19–136. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinokur, A., and E. Burnstein. 1974. Effects of partially shared persuasive arguments on group-induced shifts: A group-problem-solving approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 29(3): 305–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsy, L. 1978. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T.D., and S.J. LaFleur. 1995. Knowing what you’ll do: Effects of analyzing reasons on self-prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68(1): 21–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hugo Mercier.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mercier, H. When Experts Argue: Explaining the Best and the Worst of Reasoning. Argumentation 25, 313–327 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-011-9222-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-011-9222-y

Keywords

Navigation