Skip to main content
Log in

Physiological Correlates of the Big 5: Autonomic Responses to Video Presentations

  • Published:
Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Personality’s link to emotional experience has been demonstrated, but specific biological responses to emotion as a function of personality have not been well-established. Here, the association between personality and physiological responses (heart rate, skin conductance, and respiration) to emotional videos was assessed. One-hundred sixty-nine participants self-reported on their Big 5 personality traits and underwent ambulatory monitoring as they watched four brief video clips from primetime television content showing scenes containing violence, fear, sadness, and tension. Generally, the negatively-toned emotional scenes provoked increases in skin conductance response and declines in heart rate. We found that physiological outcomes depended on the particular emotional scene and on personality, most notably Extraversion and Neuroticism. Extraversion, and to a lesser degree, Neuroticism, were associated with increases in autonomic arousal responses to the scenes. Gender also interacted with personality to predict responses, such that women who scored higher on measures of Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Conscientious tended to show more physiological arousal than men. Overall, the emotional scenes evoked increases in arousal and more controlled attention. The findings are discussed in context of the limited capacity model and shed light on how personality and gender affect physiological reactions to emotional experiences in everyday life.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Age did not moderate any of our results.

  2. Participants rated how much they liked the specific House episode they were shown in the current study on a scale of 1 (poor) to 5 (excellent). Overall, participants enjoyed the episode; M = 4.24, SD = .83. More Agreeable people reported liking the episode more; r = .16, p < .05.

  3. In order to assure that multiple testing problems were not present, and following Sherman and Funder (2009), randomization tests were performed for each of the Big 5 traits. These tests demonstrated that the reported significant findings were greater than the number of findings that would be expected by chance.

  4. In our examination of whether physiological responses varied according to amount of Fox television watched, we found one significant correlation. The number of Fox programming hours watched per week correlated .16 with HR change to the violence-fear scene (p < .05).

References

  • Amin, Z., Constable, R., & Canli, T. (2004). Attentional bias for valenced stimuli as a function of personality in the dot-probe task. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 15–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Attansio, P., Jacobs, K., Shore, D., Singer, B., Moran, T. L., Friend, R., et al. (2008). Last resort [House]. New York City: Fox Network.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology, 37, 204–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1999). The affect system: Architecture and operating characteristics. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 133–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Tassinary, L. G. (1990). Principles of psychophysiology: Physical, social, and inferential elements. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canli, T., Amin, Z., Haas, B., Omura, K., & Constable, R. (2004). A double dissociation between mood states and personality traits in the anterior cingulate. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118, 897–904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Canli, T., Sivers, H., Whitfield, S. L., Gotlib, I. H., & Gabrieli, J. E. (2002). Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science, 296, 2191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. E. (2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 33–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of extraversion and neuroticism on subjective well-being: Happy and unhappy people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 668–678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishman, I., Ng, R., & Bellugi, U. (2011). Do extraverts process social stimuli differently from introverts? Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 67–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., Raine, A., Loeber, S., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Steinhauer, S. R. (2002). Serious delinquent behavior, sensation-seeking and electrodermal arousal. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 477–486.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. R. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A. (1971). The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behavior Research and Therapy, 8, 249–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, M. K., Cook, E. W., & Lang, P. J. (1989). Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 51–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J. (2008). Emotion regulation. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 497–512). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1984). Nonverbal sex differences: Communication accuracy and expressive style. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kossowska, M., & Nęcka, E. (1994). Do it your own way: Cognitive strategies, intelligence, and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 33–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krcmar, M., & Kean, L. G. (2005). Uses and gratifications of media violence: Personality correlates of viewing and liking violent genres. Media Psychology, 7, 399–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreibig, S. D., Wilhelm, F. H., Roth, W. T., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Cardiovascular, electrodermal, and respiratory response patterns to fear- and sadness-inducing films. Psychophysiology, 44, 787–806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, A. (2000). The limited capacity model of mediated message processing. Journal of Communication, 50, 46–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, A., Bolls, P., Potter, R., & Kawahara, K. (1999). The effects of production pacing and arousing content on the information processing of television messages. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 43, 451–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, A., Chung, Y., Lee, S., Schwartz, N., & Shin, M. (2005). It’s an arousing, fast-paced kind of world: The effects of age and sensation seeking on the information processing of substance-abuse PSAs. Media Psychology, 7, 421–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Ohman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 137–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malatesta, C. Z., Culver, C., Tesman, J., & Shepard, B. (1989). The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50, 1–2.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClure, E. (2000). A meta-analytic review in sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 424–453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1991). Adding Liebe und Arbeit: The full five-factor model and well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 227–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFatter, R. M. (1998). Emotional intensity: Some components and their relations to extraversion and neuroticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 747–758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ng, W., & Diener, E. (2009). Feeling bad? The ‘power’ of positive thinking may not apply to everyone. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 455–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, C. J., Larsen, J. T., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Neuroticism is associated with larger and more prolonged electrodermal responses to emotionally evocative pictures. Psychophysiology, 44, 823–826.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmann, S., Hopp, H., & Quirin, M. (2008). Gender differences in psychophysiological responses to disgust. Journal of Psychophysiology, 22, 65–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U., & Derryberry, D. (2005). Attentional interference effects of emotional pictures: Threat, negativity, or arousal? Emotion, 5, 55–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwebel, D. C., & Suls, J. (1999). Cardiovascular reactivity and neurotiscm: Results from a laboratory and controlled ambulatory stress protocol. Journal of Personality, 67, 67–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, R. A., & Funder, D. C. (2009). Evaluating correlations in studies of personality and behavior: Beyond the number of significant findings to be expected by chance. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 1053–1063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Thase, M. E., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2002). Can’t shake that feeling: fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 693–707.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May I have your attention, please: Electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 41, 171–183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smits, D. J. M., & De Boeck, P. (2006). From BIS/BAS to the big five. European Journal of Personality, 20, 255–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stellar, J. E., Manzo, V. M., Kraus, M. W., & Keltner, D. (2011). Class and compassion: Socioeconomic factors predict responses to suffering. Emotion, 12, 449–459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turpin, G. (1986). Effects of stimulus intensity on autonomic responding: The problem of differentiating orienting and defense reflexes. Psychophysiology, 23, 1–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., & Clark, L. (1992). On traits and temperament: General and specific factors of emotional experience and their relation to the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 441–476.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wild, B., Erb, M., & Bartels, M. (2001). Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: quality, quantity, time course and gender differences. Psychiatry Research, 102, 109–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshino, A., Kimura, Y., Yoshida, T., Takahashi, Y., & Nomura, S. (2005). Relationships between temperament dimensions in personality and unconscious emotional responses. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zelenski, J. M., & Larsen, R. J. (1999). Susceptibility to affect: A comparison of three personality taxonomies. Journal of Personality, 67, 761–791.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (2005). Psychobiology of personality (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Claudia Chloe Brumbaugh.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brumbaugh, C.C., Kothuri, R., Marci, C. et al. Physiological Correlates of the Big 5: Autonomic Responses to Video Presentations. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 38, 293–301 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-013-9234-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-013-9234-5

Keywords

Navigation