Skip to main content
Log in

The influence of context on information processing

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The main research question of this study is how the processing of information relates to different contextual characteristics. More specifically, how the context is associated with efficiency of information processing (success and speed), size of chunks, speed of chunk processing and the recall of a chunk. The research domain was the game of chess. The efficiency of information processing and the chunk characteristics were defined with the reconstruction of sequences of chess moves. Context variables were defined using a slightly adapted chess program. Variables on information dispersion, deviation, complexity and positivity were extracted in each chess position. Overall, the results showed that higher dispersion and complexity and lower positivity of information in a context lead to less efficient information processing. The results support the assumptions of the cognitive load theory about the negative effects of external factors burden on information processing and working memory. Our results also support the ACT-R theory, which suggests that more frequent information has a higher activation level and can therefore be retrieved more easily and quickly. The results are also congruent with the positivity effect, which proposes that it is easier to remember positive information than negative information. The findings of our study can be beneficial for the development of intelligent tutoring systems and the design of human–computer interaction systems.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and materials

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are included within the article and its additional files. All additional data are available upon request from the author.

Notes

  1. The best rank is marked with the lowest number, i.e., 1.

References

  • Anderson JR, Lebiere C (1998) The atomic components of thought. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson JR, Schooler LJ (1991) Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychol Sci 2:396–408

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson JR, Reder LM, Lebiere C (1996) Working memory: activation limitations on retrieval. Cogn Psychol 30(3):221–256

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bachman T, Oit M (1992) Stroop-like interference in chess players’ imagery. Psychol Res 54:27–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD, Hitch GJL (1974) Working memory. In: Bower GA (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation: advances in research and theory, vol 8. Academic Press, New York, pp 47–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD, Logie RH (1999) Working memory: the multiple-component model. In: Miyake A, Shah P (eds) Models of working memory. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 28–61

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley AD, Eysenck MW, Anderson MC (2009) Memory. Psychology Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty J (1982) Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychol Bull 91:276–292

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borenstein E, Ullman S (2008) Combined top-down/bottom-up segmentation. IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell 30(12):2109–2125

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdeau J, Grandbastien M (2010) Modeling tutoring knowledge. In: Nkambou R, Bourdeau J, Mizoguchi R (eds) Advances in intelligent tutoring system. Studies in computational intelligence, vol 308. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp 123–143

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brennan JF, Houde KA (2017) History and systems of psychology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell DJ (1988) Task complexity: a review and analysis. Acad Manag Rev 13(1):40–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen LL (1993) Motivation for social contact across the life span: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. In: Nebraska symposium on motivation vol 40, pp 209–254

  • Carstensen LL (2006) The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science 312(5782):1913–1915

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen LL, Isaacowitz DM, Charles ST (1999) Taking time seriously. A theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am Psychol 54(3):165–181

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chandler P, Sweller J (1992) The split-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction. Br J Educ Psychol 62:233–246

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chase WG, Simon HA (1973) Perception in chess. Cogn Psychol 4(1):55–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad R, Hul AJ (1964) Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. Br J Psychol 55:429–432

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crosby ME, Iding M, Chin DN (2003) Research on task complexity as a foundation for augmented cognition. In: Proceedings of the 36th Hawaii international conference on system science (HICSS-36). IEEE Computer Society 2003, Koloa, Kauai, p 132

  • Ebbinghaus H (1913) A contribution to experimental psychology. Teachers College, Columbia University, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Elo AE (1978) The rating of chess players past and present. Arco, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredericks TK, Choi SD, Hart J, Butt SE, Mital A (2005) An investigation of myocardial aerobic capacity as a measure of both physical and cognitive workloads. Int J Ind Ergon 35(12):1097–1107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gill TG, Hicks RC (2006) Task complexity and informing science: a synthesis. Inf Sci 9:1

    Google Scholar 

  • GNU Operating System (2008) GNU operating system. http://www.gnu.org. Accessed 12 May 2008

  • Gobet F, Lane PCR, Croker S, Cheng PCH, Jones G, Oliver I, Pine JM (2001) Chunking mechanisms in human learning. Trends Cognit Sci 5(6):236–243

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Godden D, Baddeley AD (1975) Context dependent memory in two natural environments. Br J Psychol 66(3):325–331

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Granholm E, Asarnow RF, Sarkin AJ, Dykes KL (1996) Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. Psychophysiology 33(4):457–461

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guérard K, Tremblay S (2012) The effect of path length and display size on memory for spatial information. Exp Psychol 59:147–152

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guérard K, Tremblay S, Saint-Aubin J (2009) The processing of spatial information in short-term memory: insights from eye tracking the path length effect. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 132(2):136–144

    Google Scholar 

  • Guid M, Mozina M, Bohak C, Sadikov A, Bratko I (2013) Building an intelligent tutoring system for chess endgames. In: Proceedings of the 5th international conference on computer supported education (CSEDU), pp 263–266

  • Hair JF, Anderson RE, Tatham RL, Black WC (1998) Multivariate data analysis, 5th edn. Prentice-Hall International, Upper Saddle River

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford GS, Maybery MT, Bain JD (1986) Capacity limitations in children’s reasoning: a dual-task approach. Child Dev 57:616–627

    Google Scholar 

  • Halford GS, Wilson WH, Phillips S (1998) Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behav Brain Sci 21(6):803–831

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Healy AF, Havas DA, Parker JT (2000) Comparing serial position effects in semantic and episodic memory using reconstruction of order tasks. J Mem Lang 42(2):147–167

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard MW, Kahana MJ (2002) A distributed representation of temporal context. J Math Psychol 46(3):269–299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hristova D, Guid M, Bratko I (2014) Assessing the difficulty of chess tactical problems. Int J Adv Intell Syst 7(3):728–738

    Google Scholar 

  • Just MA, Carpenter PA (1992) A capacity theory of comprehension. Psychol Rev 99:122–149

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalyuga S, Ayres P, Chandler P, Sweller J (2003) The expertise reversal effect. Educ Psychol 38(1):23–31

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koffka K (1935) Principles of gestalt psychology. Harcourt, Brace, New York, p 176

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler W (1971) [Original work published 1930] Human perception. In: Henle M (ed). The selected papers of Wolfgang Köhler. Liveright.: 145

  • Krivec J, Guid M, Bratko I (2009) Identification and characteristic descriptions of procedural chunks. In: Dini P (ed) Proceedings, computation world 2009, computation world: future computing, service computation, adaptive, content, cognitive, patterns. IEEE Coputer Society, New York, pp 448–453

  • Kundel HL, Nodine CF (1975) Interpreting chest radiographs without visual search. Radiology 116(3):527–532

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan GD (1988) Toward an instant theory of automatization. Pyschol Rev 95:492–527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mather M, Carstensen LL (2005) Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends Cogn Sci 9(10):496–502

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno R, Mayer R (1999) Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: the role of modality and contiguity. J Educ Psychol 91(2):358–368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Možina M, Guid M, Sadikov A, Groznik V, Bratko I (2012) Goal-oriented conceptualization of procedural knowledge. Intell Tutor Syst (ITS) 7315:286–291

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navon D, Miller JO (1987) Role of outcome conflict in dual task interference. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:438–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paas F (1992) Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: a cognitive-load approach. J Educ Psychol 84(4):429–434

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parmentier FBR, Elford G, Maybery MT (2005) Transitional information in spatial serial memory: path characteristics affect recall performance. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cognit 31:412–427

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rajlich V (2008) Rybkachess.com [Computer program]. http://www.rybkachess.com/. Accessed 22 Feb 2008

  • Reed AE, Carstensen LL (2012) The theory behind the age-related positivity effect. Front Psychol 3:339

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson P (2001) Task Complexity, task difficulty, and task production: exploring interactions in a componential framework. Appl Linguist 22(1):27–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roetzel PG (2018) Information overload in the information age: a review of the literature from business administration, business psychology, and related disciplines with a bibliometric approach and framework development. Bus Res 11:1–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Saariluoma P, Laine T (2001) Novice construction of chess memory. Scand J Psychol 42:137–146

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sadikov A, Mozina M, Guid M, Krivec J, Bratko I (2006) Automated chess tutor. Comput Games 4630:13–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharifian F (2002) Memory enhancement in language pedagogy: implications from cognitive research. TESL-EJ Electron J Engl Second Lang 6(2):1–9

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith S, Vela E (2001) Environmental context-dependent memory: a review and meta-analysis. Psychon Bull Rev 8(2):203–220

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Speck O, Ernst T, Braun J, Koch C, Miller E, Chang L (2000) Gender differences in the functional organization of the brain for working memory. NeuroReport 11:2581–2585

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoiljkovikj S, Bratko I, Guid M (2015) A computational model for estimating the difficulty of chess problems. In: Proceedings of the third annual conference on advances in cognitive systems ACS, p 7

  • Strayer DL, Cooper JM, Turrill J, Coleman JR, Hopman RJ (2016) Talking to your car can drive you to distraction. Cognit Res Princ Implic. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-016-0018-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller J (1994) Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty and instructional design. Learn Instr 4:295–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller J, Chandler P (1994) Why is some material difficult to learn? Cognit Instr 12:185–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Towse JN, Hitch GJ, Hutton U (2000) On the interpretation of working memory span in adults. Memory Cognit 28:341–348

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E, Pearlstone Z (1966) Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 5(4):381–391

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Von Restorff H (1933) Über die wirkung von bereichsbildungen im spurenfeld. Psychologische Forschung 18(1):299–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver W (1948) Science and Complexity. Am Sci 36(4):536–544

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer M (1923) Untersuchungen zur Lehre von der Gestalt. II. Psychol Res 4(1): 301–350 (Translated extract reprinted as “Laws of organization in perceptual forms”. In Ellis WD (ed) (1938) A source book of Gestalt psychology. Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd., London, pp 71–94)

  • Woolf BP (2010) Building intelligent interactive tutors: student-centered strategies for revolutionizing e-learning. Morgan Kaufmann, Burlington

    Google Scholar 

  • Zacks RT, Hasher L, Li KZH (2008) The handbook of aging and cognition, 3rd edn. Psychology Press, East Sussex

    Google Scholar 

  • Zoelch C, Schumann-Hengsteler R (2006) Aspects of complexity in visual-spatial working memory: indication for the application of strategies? Cognit Process 7(1):165

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Contributions to the research should be acknowledged to Dr. Ivan Bratko, head of the Artificial Intelligence Laboratory of The Faculty of Computer and Information Science of Ljubljana University, the co-mentor of the doctoral dissertation in which the research was performed, or the scientific guidance. We would like to express my gratitude to all the chess players who cooperate in the research. We thank the anonymous reviewers for their careful reading of our manuscript and their many insightful comments and suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jana Krivec.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee (Etična komisija FUDŠ) and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Handling editor: Luigia Carlucci Aiello (Sapienza University of Rome).

Reviewers: Tejaswinee Kelkar (University of Oslo) and a researcher who prefers to remain anonymous.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 3218 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Krivec, J., Guid, M. The influence of context on information processing. Cogn Process 21, 167–184 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-020-00958-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-020-00958-8

Keywords

Navigation