Skip to main content
Log in

Studies on time: a proposal on how to get out of circularity

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The analysis of time is vitiated very often by circularity: several disciplines, such as psychology, linguistics, and neurosciences, analyze time by using concepts or terms which already contain in themselves, or are based, on the experience and notion of time (as when, for example, time is defined as “duration”, or when our ability to estimate durations is explained by resorting to the notion of an internal clock). Some detailed examples of circularity in the analysis of time are given here and examined. A way out of circularity is then given: it is represented by the proposal of attentional semantics (AS) of considering words and their meanings in terms of the aim they serve, and the means and processes developed and implemented in order to achieve that aim. According to AS, the main aim of words is that of indicating to, and eliciting in, the listener or reader a specific conscious experience: namely, the conscious experience referred to by their meanings. Words achieve their main aim by conveying the condensed instructions on the attentional operations one has to perform if one wants to consciously experience what is expressed through and by them. By describing the conscious experiences elicited by words in terms of the attentional operations that are responsible for the production of such conscious experiences, AS offers an a-linguistic counterpart to language, and therefore an effective way out of circularity. Following in footsteps of Mach (Contributions to the analysis of the sensations, 1890), but slightly revising his hypothesis, AS defines time-sensation as the perception of the effort made, or alternatively the nervous energy expended, by the organ of attention when performing a “temporal activity” (for instance, estimating duration), that is, when one’s own attention is focused in a continuous and incremental way on the conscious product of the (“non-temporal”) activity performed by means of another portion of one’s attention.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Notes

  1. Four different psychophysical methods were used to assess prospective time judgments: the method of constant stimuli, the method of magnitude estimation, the method of stimulus duration reproduction, and the method of single stimuli.

References

  • Angrilli A, Cherubini P, Pavese A, Manfredini S (1997) The influence of affective factors on time perception. Percept psychophy 59(6):972–982

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baars BJ (1988) A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Benedetti G (1999) La categoria di ‘spazio’. In AAVV (ed) Studi in memoria di Silvio Ceccato. Società Stampa Sportiva, Roma

  • Benedetti G (2008) I meccanismi del pensiero e del linguaggio. Una barriera invalicabile? (in press)

  • Benussi V (1913) Psychologie der Zeitauffassung. Winter, Heidelberg

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz A (1997) Le sens du mouvement. Odile Jacob, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Block RA (1979) Time and Consciousness. In: Underwood G, Stevens R (eds) Aspects of consciousness, vol I: psychological issues. Academic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Block RA (1982) Temporal judgments and contextual change. J Exp Psychol Learn mem Cogn 8:530–544

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Block RA (1986) Remembered duration: imagery processes and contextual encoding. Acta Psychol 62:103–122

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Block RA (1990) Model of psychological time. In: Block RA (ed) Cognitive models of psychological time. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Block RA (2003) Psychological timing without a timer: the roles of attention and memory. In: Helfrich H (ed) Time and mind II: information processing perspectives. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers, Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  • Block RA, Reed MA (1978) Remembered duration: evidence for a contextual-change hypothesis. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem 4:656–665

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block RA, Zakay D (1996) Models of psychological time revisited. In: Helfrich H (ed) Time and mind. Hogrefe & Huber Publishers, Seattle

    Google Scholar 

  • Block RA, Zakay D (2001) Retrospective and prospective timing: memory, attention, and consciousness. In: Hoerl C, McCormack T (eds) Time and memory. Issues in philosophy and psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom P (2000) How children learn the meanings of words. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolles EB (2006) Babel’s Dawn. http://ebbolles.typepad.com/babels_dawn

  • Bozzi P, Vicario GB (1960) Due fattori di unificazione fra note musicali: la vicinanza temporale e la vicinanza tonale. Riv Psicol 54:235–258

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown SW (1985) Time perception and attention: the effects of prospective versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration. Percept Psychophys 38:115–124

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown SW, Stubbs DA (1992) Attention and interference in prospective and retrospective timing. Perception 21:545–557

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen K-U (2002) Location and distance in language: an attention-based approach. Paper presented at the 3rd Annual Language & Space Workshop ‘Resolution and Granularity in Language and Space’. ZIF, Univ. Bielefeld. 8./9.7.2002

  • Carstensen K-U (2007) Spatio-temporal ontologies and attention. Spat Cogn Comput 7(1):13–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceccato S (1968) Cibernetica per tutti, vol I. Feltrinelli, Milano

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceccato S (ed) (1969) Corso di linguistica operativa. Longanesi, Milano

  • Ceccato S (1970) Cibernetica per tutti, vol II. Feltrinelli, Milano

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceccato S (1972) La mente vista da un cibernetico. Eri, Torino

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceccato S, Zonta B (1980) Linguaggio consapevolezza pensiero. Feltrinelli, Milano

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland PS, Sejinowski TJ (1992) The computational brain. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Coull JT, Vidal F, Nazarian B, Macar F (2004) Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Science 303:1506–1508

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M (1992) Flow. The classic work on how to achieve happiness. Rider, Random House Group, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett DC, Kinsbourne M (1992) Time and the observer: the where and when of consciousness in the brain. Behav Brain Sci 15(2):183–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey J (1933) How we think. Heat, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Dingler H (1938) Die methode der Physik. Monaco

  • Duncan J (1984) Selective attention and the organization of visual information. J Exp Psychol Gen 113:501–517

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eagleman DM, Tse PU, Buonomano D, Janssen P, Nobre AC, Holcombe AO (2005) Time and the brain: how subjective time relates to neural time. J Neurosci 25(45):10369–10371

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Evans V (2004) The structure of time. Language, meaning and temporal cognition. John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Fagot C, Pashler H (1992) Making two responses to a single object: implications for the central attentional bottleneck. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:1058–1079

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ferretti F (1996) I linguaggi del pensiero. Le forme della rappresentazione mentale. In: Gambarara D (ed) Pensiero e linguaggio. Introduzione alle ricerche contemporanee. La Nuova Italia Scientifica, Roma

    Google Scholar 

  • Fingelkurts AA, Fingelkurts AA (2006) Timing in cognition and EEG brain dynamics: discreteness versus continuity. Cogn Process 7:135–162

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flaherty M (1999) A watched pot: how we experience time. New York University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse P (1963) The psychology of time. Harper & Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraser JT (1987) Tim. The familiar stranger. The University of Massachusetts Press, Amherst

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman WJ (1993) Memory for the time of past events. Psychol Bull 113:44–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman WJ (2001) Memory processes underlying humans’ chronological sense of the past. In: Hoerl C, McCormack T (eds) Time and memory, issues in philosophy and psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman WJ, Wilkins AJ (1985) Scale effects in memory for the time of events. Mem Cogn 13:168–175

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson J (1975) Events are perceivable but time is not. In: Fraser JT, Lawrence N (eds) The study of time II. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson J (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • von Glasersfeld E (1985) Reconstructing the concept of knowledge. Arch Psychol 53:91–101

    Google Scholar 

  • Glicksohn J (2001) Temporal cognition and the phenomenology of time: a multiplicative function for apparent duration. Conscious Cogn 10:1–25

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goodson FE (2003) The evolution and function of cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadamard J (1949) The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Haikonen PO (2003) The cognitive approach to conscious machines. Imprint Academic, Exeter

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks RE, Miller GW, Kinsbourne M (1976) Prospective and retrospective judgments of time as a function of amount of information processed. Am J Psychol 90:431–446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hicks RE, Miller GW, Gaes G, Bierman K (1977) Concurrent processing demands and the experience of time-in-passing. Am J Psychol 89:719–730

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoagland H (1933) The physiological control of judgments of duration: evidence for a chemical clock. J Gen Psychol 9:267–287

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoagland H (1935) Pacemakers in relation to aspects of behavior. The Macmillan Co., New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (1983) Semantics and cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff R (1992) Languages of the mind. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • James W (1890) The principles of psychology. Holt, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanabus M, Szelag E, Rojek E, Pöppel E (2002) Temporal order judgement for auditory and visual stimuli. Acta Neurobiol Exp 62:263–270

    Google Scholar 

  • La Berge D (1995) Attentional processing. The brain’s art of mindfulness. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladefoged P, Broadbent DE (1960) Perception of sequence in auditory events. Q J Exp Psychol 13:325–349

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff G, Johnson M (1999) Philosophy in the flesh: the embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Large EW, Jones MR (1999) The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychol Rev 106(1):119–159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie N (1995) Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21:451–468

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux JE (1995) Emotion: clues from the brain. Annu Rev Psychol 46:209–235

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Macar F, Grondin S, Casini L (1994) Controlled attention sharing influences time estimation. Mem cogn 22:673–686

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mach E (1883) Die Mechanik in ihrer Entwickelung historisch-kritisch dargestellt. Brockhaus, Leipzig

    Google Scholar 

  • Mach E (1890) Contributions to the analysis of the sensations. The Open Court Publishing Company, La Salle

    Google Scholar 

  • Mack A, Rock I (1998) Inattentional Blindness. Bradford Book. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti G (1993) The mechanics of the mind. Espansione, Roma

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti G (1994) A mind theory for the human-centredness paradigm. AI Soc 8:363–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti G (2001) A theory of consciousness. http://www.mind-consciousness-language.com

  • Marchetti G (2003) Foundations of attentional semantics. http://www.mind-consciousness-language.com

  • Marchetti G (2005) The importance of non-attentional operations for attentional semantics. http://www.mind-consciousness-language.com

  • Marchetti G (2006a) A presentation of attentional semantics. Cogn Process 7:163–194

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti G (2006b) Commentary on Vyvyan Evan’s the structure of time. Language, meaning and temporal cognition. http://www.mind-consciousness-language.com

  • Marchetti G (2007) Time is energy. A hypothesis on the attentional origin of the conscious experience of time. http://www.mind-consciousness-language.com

  • Mauk MD, Buonomano DV (2004) The neural basis of temporal processing. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:307–340

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod PD (1977) A dual task response modality effect: support for multi-processor models of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 29:651–667

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLeod PD (1978) Does probe RT measure central processing demand? Q J Exp Psychol 30:83–89

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mesulam MM (1990) Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann Neurol 28:597–613

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miller GA (1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev 63:81–97

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nakayama K, Mackeben M (1989) Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vis Res 29:1631–1647

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Navon D (1984) Resources—a theoretical soup stone? Psychol Rev 91:216–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Negrotti M (1999) The theory of the artificial. Intellect Books, Exeter

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman A, Soares JJF (1994) ‘Unconscious anxiety’: phobic responses to masked stimuli. J Abnorm Psychol 103:231–240

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein RE (1969) On the experience of time. Penguin Books, Middlesex

    Google Scholar 

  • Pashler HE (1989) Dissociations and dependencies between speed and accuracy: evidence for a two-component theory of divided attention in simple task. Cogn Psychol 21:469–514

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler HE (1998) The psychology of attention. Massachusetts Institute of technology, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Penton-Voak IS, Edwards Percival A, Wearden JH (1996) Speeding up an internal clock in humans? Effects of click trains on subjective duration. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Processes 22:307–320

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pöppel E (2004) Lost in time: a historical frame, elementary processing units and the 3-second window. Acta Neurobiol Exp 64:295–301

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI (1990) Hierarchical distributed networks in the neuropsychology of selective attention. In: Caramazza A (ed) Cognitive neuropsychology and neurolinguistics. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI (1995) Attention in cognitive neuroscience: an overview. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) Handbook of cognitive neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 13:25–42

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn ZW (1984) Computation and cognition. Toward a foundation for cognitive science. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Richelle M, Lejeune H, Perikel J-J, Fery P (1985) From biotemporality to nootemporality: toward an integrative and comparative view of time in behaviour. In: Michon JA, Jackson JL (eds) Time, mind, and behaviour. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin E (1949) Esperimenta psychologica. Munskgaard, Copenhagen

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore DI, Spence C, Klein RM (2001) Visual prior entry. Psychol Sci 12:205–212

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shore DI, Spence C (2004) Prior Entry. In: Itti L, Rees G, Tsotsos J (eds) Neurobiology of attention. Elsevier, North Holland

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy L (2000) Toward a cognitive semantics, volume I: concept structuring system. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor S (2005) The sources of higher states of consciousness. Int J Transpers Stud 24:48–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor S (2007) Making time. Why time seems to pass at different speeds and how to control it. Icon Books Ltd, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas EAC, Brown TI Jr (1974) Time perception and the filled-duration illusion. Percept Psychophys 16:449–458

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas EAC, Weaver WB (1975) Cognitive processing and time perception. Percept Psychophy 17:363–367

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman M (1963) Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: implications for a model of the ‘internal clock’. Psychological Monographs 77(whole no. 576):1–31

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman M (1984) Temporal rhythms and cerebral rhythms. In: Gibbon J, Allen L (eds) Timing and time perception. New York Academy of Sciences, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Tse PU, Intriligator J, Rivest J, Cavanagh P (2004) Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Percept Psychophys 66(7):1171–1189

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaccarino G (1974) La mente vista in operazioni. G. d’Anna, Messina-Firenze

  • Vaccarino G (1981) Analisi dei significati. Armando Armando, Roma

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela FJ, Toro A, John ER, Schwartz EL (1981) Perceptual framing and cortical alpha rhythm. Neuropsychologia 19:675–686

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vicario GB (1963) “La ‘dislocazione temporale’ nella percezione di successioni di stimoli discreti. Riv Psicol 57:17–87

    Google Scholar 

  • Vicario GB (1965) Vicinanza spaziale e vicinanza temporale nella segregazione degli eventi. Riv Psicol 59:843–863

    Google Scholar 

  • Vicario GB (2005) Il tempo. Saggio di psicologia sperimentale. Il Mulino, Bologna

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward LM (2003) Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends Cogn Sci 7(12):553–559

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wearden JH (2001) Internal clocks and the representation of time. In: Hoerl C, McCormack T (eds) Time and memory. Issues in philosophy and psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Wearden JH, Edwards H, Fakhri M, Percival A (1998) Why ‘sounds are judged longer than lights’: application of a model of the internal clock in humans. Q J Exp Psychol 51B:97–120

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener N (1948) Cybernetics, or control and communication in the animal and the machine. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Wierzbicka A (1996) Semantics. Primes and universals. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright K (2006) Times of our lives. Sci Am 16(1):26–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt W (1902) Grundzüge der physiologischen psychologie. Engelmann, Leipzig

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyss R, König P, Verschure PFMJ (2002) Invariant encoding of spatial stimulus topology in the temporal domain. Neurocomputing 44–46:703–708

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyss R, König P, Verschure PFMJ (2003) Invariant representations of visual patterns in a temporal population code. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100(1):324–329

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zakay D (1998) Attention allocation policy influences prospective timing. Psychono Bull Rev 5:114–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Zakay D, Block RA (1997) Temporal cognition. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 6(1):12–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakay D, Block RA (2004) Prospective and retrospective duration judgments: an executive-control perspective. Acta Neurobiol Exp 64:319–328

    Google Scholar 

  • Zakay D, Tsal Y (1989) Awareness of attention allocation and time estimation accuracy. Bull Psychon Soc 27:209–210

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am highly indebted to Franco Baroncini, CEO of Sincron©, for having always encouraged me to carry on my research throughout all these years. I thank Joseph Glicksohn and Peter Hancock for their insightful and constructive comments on a previous draft of the paper. I also thank my colleague Giulio Benedetti and Steve Taylor, from whom I have always had fruitful advices and suggestions. I am also particularly grateful to Nick White for having revised the English version of the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Giorgio Marchetti.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Marchetti, G. Studies on time: a proposal on how to get out of circularity. Cogn Process 10, 7–40 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-008-0215-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-008-0215-1

Keywords

Navigation