Introduction

Animals in the wild experience many different external stimuli from the environment, which can be classified as positive, neutral, or negative. Individuals are able to learn, remember, and make use of previous experiences in assessing risks and making decisions in similar situations (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). For instance, individuals often choose breeding sites that are similar to those in which they were successful in a previous year (Hoover 2003), make repeated visits to well-known, rich feeding sites (Ezaki 1992; Mettke-Hofmann and Gwinner 2003), or use the same safe stopovers during migration (Minias et al. 2010). They may also avoid inedible prey that they tasted in the past (Lynn 2005; Skelhorn et al. 2016), discriminate among different kinds of predators, and employ appropriate strategies to avoid being preyed upon and to increase breeding success (Curio 1975; Kullberg and Lind 2002; Peluc et al. 2008). However, there is justified concern that the ability of animals to acquire experience and to utilise it in future decision-making could bias the results of experimental studies. This is especially true for playback experiments that are used to study acoustic communication. Using this technique, a tested individual is usually exposed many times to various kinds of songs played back from a speaker (e.g. Jabłoński and Matyjasiak 2002; Budka and Osiejuk 2013; Osiejuk 2014). During the experiment, researchers observe and describe in detail the behaviour of the subject (McGregor 2000). It has been shown that birds decrease the intensity of their response during their second exposure to playback, even if the songs in successive treatments are different and played back from different places (Krebs 1976, Rivera-Gutierrez et al. 2017). Reduced aggressiveness after intrusion is usually observed in the short term, such as over a few hours or days (Rivera-Gutierrez et al. 2017). However, some species are also able to remember the playback experience in the longer term and may reduce the intensity of their response even 1 year after a single playback event (Rivera-Gutierrez et al. 2015). Such plasticity in behaviour is related to a learning and remembering process called “habituation”. When individuals experience a potentially threatening event for the first time (e.g. songs played back from a speaker), they respond aggressively. However, when the same threat is repeated many times without consequences for the territory holder, individuals perceive it as non-threatening or irrelevant and respond less intensely in subsequent situations (Dong and Clayton 2009; Harris and Haskell 2013).

Ornithologists often capture birds from the wild during field studies. After capture, birds can be subjected to multiple actions that differ greatly in their invasiveness, such as: taking biometric measurements and weighing; marking with alphanumerical, metal, or colour rings; and blood sampling (e.g. Burton et al. 2006; Ręk et al. 2011; Fourcade et al. 2016). These activities are often necessary to discover and explore the biology and ecology of a species. However, such stressful events related to direct contact with a human could have both short- and long-term effects on individuals, and may alter birds’ behaviour after capture (Dubiec 2011; Linhart et al. 2012). Therefore, it is important to know the extent of the influence of capturing and handling on birds’ subsequent behaviour. To date, this question has been addressed in only a few studies. For example, Camacho et al. (2017) found that Pied Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) are more difficult to catch when they have already experienced trapping, which is particularly relevant to the correct interpretation of capture-recapture data. A similar result was reported for the Great Tit (Parus major), in which individuals captured in their nest boxes were more vigilant and hesitated longer before entering nest boxes than individuals that had not been captured (Seress et al. 2017). Linhart et al. (2012) found that capture-experienced males of the Willow Warbler (Phylloscopus trochillus) showed increased response latencies to a playback lure compared to inexperienced birds. Moreover, birds have been shown to distinguish the intentions of a researcher and escape from various distances, depending on whether the researcher approaches them directly or tangentially (Møller and Tryjanowski 2014). Furthermore, Magpies (Pica pica) are even able to distinguish among individual humans and respond more aggressively toward observers who posed a threat to their nests in the past than towards observers who have not previously behaved in a threatening way (Lee et al. 2011). All of the above examples prove that birds learn from negative experiences with humans and that they subsequently increase their vigilance in similar, risky situations. Evolutionarily speaking, these are appropriate behaviours that increase chances of survival. However, this has serious implications for experimental design in biological and ecological studies, as previous experience of humans may strongly affect the response of tested individuals. Usually, experienced birds reduce the intensity of their response and are more cautious than naive ones (Linhart et al. 2012; Camacho et al. 2017). The consequence of this behavioural modification is an increase in the probability that any significant effects of the experimental treatment will not be found. Therefore, especially in the case of model species, it is necessary to conduct detailed studies on the consequences of previous experiences on bird behaviour.

In this study we focused on the cumulative effect of a previous playback lure and trapping experience on birds’ behaviour during a subsequent playback experiment. Activities such as trapping or luring by playback are commonly undertaken in ornithology, but the consequences of such approaches remain unexplored, and most of the studies using these stimuli have considered the effect of each stimulus separately (e.g. responses to playback in successive playback events or trapping probability in successive trapping approaches). As a focal species we chose the Chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs, a socially monogamous and territorial species, and one of the most common European songbirds. We experimentally examined whether the behaviour of adult (in their second calendar year or older) territorial Chaffinch males during the playback experiment depended on their previous playback and trapping experience. First, we taught individual birds to associate playback with capture by a human, using mist nets to trap birds. Then we performed another playback experiment with a dummy Chaffinch specimen to examine differences in behaviour between the previously captured birds and naive birds with no experience of capture. Thus, in our experiment, birds utilized experiences gained in one context (luring by a playback to the mist net) in another context (simulated intrusion to the territory by a foreign male). We predicted that males that had been previously lured by playback and caught by a mist net (experienced males) would delay their approach and stay further away from the speaker during the subsequent playback experiment compared to males without such experience (naive males). To the best of our knowledge, our study is the second to consider the cumulative effects of a playback lure, capturing and handling on the subsequent behaviour of birds during a playback experiment.

Methods

Study site

We conducted our study from 13 April to 15 May 2017. The study site comprised a continuous complex (approximately 3.7 km2) of public forests in the suburbs of the city of Warsaw (52°18′58″N, 20°54′50″E), Poland. From the east, the study area borders the Vistula river, while in the other directions it is surrounded by the city. A two-lane road divides the study area into two parts and generates some traffic noise. The main forest types were mixed, multi-storey stands and monocultures of Scots pine (Pinus silvestris).

Capture procedure

We used playback of Chaffinch songs to lure birds, then captured and ringed them. We then performed another playback experiment with both ringed and non-ringed birds to measure their responses to a simulated intruder in their territory. We played back the songs of five different males (25 songs per male), repeated in random order, to catch birds. Birds were captured using ornithological mist nets (Ecotone). A mist net was placed within the territory of a male, the speaker was hung near the net (without a decoy specimen), and a Chaffinch song was played back (we used five different types of songs played randomly; these songs were not used in subsequent playback trials) to attract the male. Capture was not successful in each territory, as we were not able to catch ca. 20–25% of the males that we approached. Each captured male was measured, weighed with an electronic balance, and marked with a metal alpha-numerical ornithological ring as well as a plastic coloured ring. Samples of blood and feathers were taken, and the male was released in the place of capture. The handling procedure lasted ca. 25 min and all males were released in good condition.

Playback experiment design

We tested whether capture-experienced Chaffinch males responded differently than naive males toward intrusion within their territory using playback of a foreign male. Territories of experienced and naive males were visited 1–4 days before the experiment to determine the exact territorial boundaries. For experienced males, the playback experiment was conducted between 2 and 8 days after capture. During the playback experiment, we set a pole (1.5 m high) in the middle of the target male’s territory with a dummy specimen of a Chaffinch male on top. We used four different dummy males. The speaker (JBL Flip 3; 2 × 8 W, 85 Hz–20 kHz, signal-to-noise ratio < 80 dB, black colour) was placed below the dummy male. We also set eight poles around the speaker (four poles 4 m from the speaker and four poles 2 m from the speaker) to better determine the distance between the speaker and the tested male. The playback was turned on (Sony Xperia Z3 connected to the speaker by Bluetooth) only when we were sure that the focal male was present in the territory and located less than 20 m from the speaker. The amplitude of the playback was set at 81 ± 3 dB at 1 m, which corresponds to the average level observed in naturally singing Chaffinch males (Brumm and Ritschard 2011). The experiment comprised three phases (3 min each): pre-playback, playback, and post-playback (see Supplementary material S1). During the pre-playback and post-playback phase we observed and recorded males’ behaviour (for details see below); in the playback phase songs of male Chaffinches were played back with an interval of 7 s between songs. We used songs of 23 different males (one song per male repeated many times; see Supplementary materials S2) that had been recorded 250–300 km from the study area; we may thus assume that the tested males were not familiar with the experimental songs. Different random combinations of song (one of 23) and dummy Chaffinch male (one of four) were used to test 23 experienced males and 23 naive males. Experienced and naive males were tested in random order; we therefore observed no differences in the stage of the season or the time of day when the experiment was performed for either group of birds. Each experiment was recorded using a Marantz PMD661 digital recorder connected to a Sennheiser ME67 shotgun microphone with a K6 powering module (48 kHz, 16-bit sampling). Two observers (different from those who had captured birds) were located in two different points ca. 15 m from the speaker during the experiment, to better describe the tested male’s behaviour. Playback experiments were conducted from 18 April to 15 May (on average 1 May ± 7 days), between 0451 and 1023 hours (on average 0711 hours ± 85 min; local time).

Measures of males’ responses to playback

The males’ behaviour was observed and recorded by a digital recorder during the experiment. Recordings were analysed using Avisoft SasLab Pro software. As measures of the response to playback we used the following vocal characteristics: (1) number of songs, (2) number of “chink” calls (calls uttered during territorial fights or while mobbing ground predators), and (3) number of “hreet” calls (specialised calls uttered mainly in the context of territorial defence or signalling ownership) (Marler 1956) (see Figs. 1; S1). We also recorded characteristics of bird movements during the experiment: (4) number of flights, and (5) minimum distance to the speaker (ordinal variable with three categories: less than 2 m, 2–4 m, more than 4 m). Additionally, we measured (6) the latency to the first vocal response (song or call), and (7) latency to the first flight. Latency-related responses were defined as the time (in seconds) from the beginning of the first song of playback to the first vocalisation or first flight; thus a lower value indicated a faster approach. Finally, during each experiment, observers assessed whether a female was in sight. Since both males and females produce chink calls, the microphone was always directed at the focal male. If a female started calling, the observer recorded which calls belonged to the male and which to the female. In this way we minimised the probability that a female’s calls would be classified as a male’s.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Three measures of the acoustic response of Chaffinch males: song, “hreet” calls, and “chink” calls. Spectrogram were generated in Avisoft Sas Lab Pro software with the following settings: fast Fourier transform = 512, frame size = 75%, windows = hamming, overlap = 75%. For the original recording, see Supplementary material (S1)

Statistical analyses

We analysed each measure of male behaviour separately to examine in detail which behavioural variables of the tested males were modified by their previous experience of a playback lure and human capture. First, we examined whether experienced and naive males differed in behaviour during the pre-playback phase, when only observers were present within a territory (Mann–Whitney test). Next, we investigated how intrusion by a foreign male (simulated by playback) modified the behaviour of the territory holder independently of their experience of capture by humans. We compared changes in a given male’s behaviour during the three phases of the experiment—pre-playback, playback, and post-playback—using generalised estimating equations (GEE). We constructed a separate GEE model for each of the measures of males’ behaviour that could be observed in each phase of the experiment: (1) the number of songs, (2) the number of chink calls, (3) the number of hreet calls, and (4) the number of flights. Dependent variables were fitted using a negative binomial distribution and log link function. The predictor variables were: (1) phase of the experiment (categorical predictor), (2) date when the experiment was conducted (continuous predictor), (3) hour when the experiment started (continuous predictor), and (4) the presence of a female during the experiment (categorical predictor). We used main effect models with intercepts included in the models. We based the covariance matrix on a robust estimator and we specified the working correlation matrix (i.e. within-subject dependencies) as independent (i.e. repeated measurements are uncorrelated).

Differences in behaviour between capture-experienced and naive males during the playback phase were analysed using generalised linear models. We constructed separate models for each of the measures of male behaviour: (1) the number of songs, (2) the number of chink calls, (3) the number of hreet calls, (4) the number of flights, (5) latency to the first vocal response, (6) latency to the first flight, and (7) and minimum distance to the speaker. As predictors we used: (1) treatment (categorical predictor), (2) presence of a female during the experiment (categorical predictor), (3) date when the experiment was conducted (continuous predictor), and (4) hour when the experiment started (continuous predictor). We fitted data using a negative binomial distribution and log link function (predictors 1–4), a gamma distribution and log link function (predictors 5–6), and a multinomial distribution with cumulative probit link function (predictor 7). Analogous models were run for the post-playback phase. However, for these we considered only the first four measures of male behaviour, since only those measures could be observed at this stage of the experiment.

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS software version 24. When necessary, the normality of variables was examined using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. All p-values are two-tailed. We applied a Holm–Bonferroni correction to counteract the problem of multiple comparisons (Holm 1979).

Results

Male behaviour during pre-playback phase

We tested 46 Chaffinch males—23 experienced and 23 naive individuals—to examine whether previous playback and capture experiences affected birds’ behaviour during a subsequent playback experiment. We did not find significant differences in behaviour between experienced and naive males during the 3-min pre-playback phase (Table 1). During this phase males usually spent time in one place (performed from zero to three flights), sung on average eight songs (range 0–25), uttered a few hreet calls (on average 5.5, range 0–122) and even fewer chink calls (on average 1.5, range 0–36).

Table 1 Comparison of behaviour of experienced and naive male Chaffinches during the 3-min pre-playback phase

The effect of playback on male behaviour

The tested males significantly modified their behaviour in response to playback. During the playback phase, males significantly increased the number of flights and the number of chink calls, while simultaneously decreasing the number of songs sung with respect to both the pre-playback and post-playback phase (Table 2). Hreet calls were significantly more frequent during the post-playback phase than during the playback phase. All analysed measures of male response correlated weakly with each other (Pearson correlation; songs and hreet calls, r = − 0.238, p = 0.005; songs and chink calls, r = − 0.284, p = 0.001; songs and flights, r = − 0.268, p = 0.001; hreet calls and chink calls, r = 0.136, r = 0.111; hreet calls and flights, r = − 0.006, p = 0.944; chink calls and flights, r = 0.285, p = 0.001). The effects of the presence of a female during the experiment, or the date and hour when the experiment was conducted, were not significant in any model (Table 2; Fig. 2).

Table 2 Results of four generalised estimating equations comparing the behaviour of Chaffinch males during the three phases of the experiment
Fig. 2
figure 2

Chaffinch males’ behaviour during the three phases of the experiment: pre-playback, playback, and post-playback. Mean values and SEM are given. Experienced and naive males are considered together

The effect of experience on male behaviour during the playback phase

Experienced males differed in behaviour compared to naive males during the 3-min playback phase. Experienced males sang less often and were less likely to approach closely to the speaker (< 2 m; result not significant after Holm–Bonferroni correction) compared to naive males (Table 3; Figs. 3, 4). Instead, experienced males produced significantly more calls (hreet and chink) than naive males (Table 3; Fig. 3). We did not find any significant differences in the number of flights or the latency to vocal and flight responses between experienced and naive males (Table 3; Fig. 3). The number of hreet calls significantly decreased over the course of the day, while the number of chink calls increased. The number of songs in this phase of the experiment decreased with the progression of both the season and the day; however, the effects of hour and stage of season were very small compared to the capture-experience effect and not significant after applying Holm–Bonferroni correction (Table 3). We found only three significant correlations between analysed measures of males’ responses (Pearson correlation; number of songs and number of chink calls, r = − 0.293, p < 0.05; number of flights and latency of flight, r = − 0.470, p < 0.001; latency of flight and latency of vocal response, r = 0.389, p < 0.05).

Table 3 Results of seven generalised linear models investigating the behaviour of male Chaffinches as a function of experience (experienced vs. naive males), the presence of a female during the test (present-absent), and the date and time (h) of the test
Fig. 3
figure 3

Differences in response during the playback phase between experienced and naive Chaffinch males. Bars denote means, whiskers denote SEs. ** p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001; for more details, see Table 3

Fig. 4
figure 4

Differences in the approach to the speaker between experienced and naive males during the playback phase. Number of males observed within each distance category (minimal distance) to the speaker are given

The effect of experience on male behaviour during the post-playback phase

Analyses of males’ behaviour during the post-playback phase showed that experienced males produced significantly more chink calls than naive males. There were no significant differences for the other three measures of birds’ responses (numbers of flights, songs, and hreet calls) between naive and experienced males (Table 4; Fig. 5). However, our models showed that males modified their behaviour depending on the presence of a female during the experiment: males accompanied by a female flew more often and produced fewer hreet calls than males around which a female was not observed during this phase of the experiment (effects not significant after Holm–Bonferroni correction). Also, the hour of the experiment had a significant but very weak effect on the number of hreet calls produced in the post-playback phase (Table 4; Fig. 5).

Table 4 Results of four generalised linear models comparing the behaviour of tested Chaffinch males during the post-playback phase
Fig. 5
figure 5

Chaffinch males’ behaviour during the post-playback phase. Only significant effects are shown (see Table 4). Differences in the number of songs (a), number of flights (b), number of “hreet” calls and number of “chink” calls between experienced and naive males are given. Mean values and SE are shown. ***p < 0.001. For more details see Table 4

Discussion

We applied a typical approach that is used in many biological and ecological studies of birds: we caught individuals, and a few days after this negative experience for the birds, we performed a playback experiment. As a control group, we used males that did not have this type of experience of humans. There were no significant differences in behaviour during the pre-playback phase between these two groups of males when the speaker, dummy male, and observers were within the territory of a target individual but the song playback had not yet begun. This suggests that both naive and experienced birds do not treat a human presence in their territory as a threat in itself, at least during the pre-playback phase. We also observed no significant differences in the number of flights and latency to the first flight or vocal response during the playback phase (3 min, during which a song of a foreign male was played back from the speaker). These two measures of a bird’s response—number of flights and latency to response—are often used to describe birds’ behaviour during playback experiments (e.g. Skierczyński et al. 2007; Brumm and Ritschard 2011), and here, these measures were independent of the previous experience of tested males. It seems that, after an acoustic intrusion, the territory holders tried to locate the intruder as fast as possible, regardless of any previous experience of lure by playback and capture by humans. However, this pattern seems to be species specific, since in the Willow Warbler, capture-experienced males increased latency to response to playback compared to naive males (Linhart et al. 2012). This difference may result from different strategies of territorial defence among species. For example, Brumm and Ritschard (2011) found that Chaffinch males spent more time within 5 m of the speaker and approached closer to the speaker when songs of lower amplitude were played back. In the same experiment they found no significant differences in the number of flights and latency of response during the playback. That indicates that Chaffinch males respond just as quickly and fly with the same intensity in response to all intruders but change their distance to the perceived threat depending on the type of intruder. The opposite pattern was observed in the Willow Warbler. In this species, males flew less and were slower to approach the speaker in response to playback of a neighbour with which they shared the border of their territory than in response to playback of a more distant male, but there was no significant difference with respect to the number of songs and latency to sing (Jaška et al. 2015).

In contrast to latency and the number of flights, other measures of Chaffinch males’ responses depended on differences in the previous experiences of tested birds. During the playback phase, experienced males did not approach as closely to the speaker as naive birds; however, this measure of response was not significant when we applied Holm–Bonferroni correction for multiple testing. Similar results have been reported in other studies that examined the influence of human-induced disturbances on birds’ later behaviour (Camacho et al. 2017; Harris and Haskell 2013; Lima and Roper 2009; Rivera-Gutierrez et al. 2015). Thus, a commonly used measure of birds’ response to playback—the time spent within a certain distance of the speaker (e.g. 5 m in Budka and Osiejuk 2013; Wei et al. 2011)—can strongly depend on a tested individual’s previous experience of capture by humans. Therefore, it is possible that the same experiment performed on experienced or naive males may result in completely different results (i.e. significant or not) when the response variable examined is, for example, a minimum-approach distance or time spent within 5 m of the speaker (Brumm and Ritschard 2011). Another problem is that the results of studies based on naive and experienced males cannot be directly compared. These are serious challenges for the planning, execution, and analysis of research based on individuals that have been captured by a researcher. The simplest way to overcome this problem is to use study sites or populations that have not been disturbed by capture or luring by playback (Rivera-Gutierrez et al. 2015). Alternatively, when playback experiments are carried out on experienced males, it may be helpful to increase the area (distance from the lure) within which the individual is recorded (e.g. time spent 5, 10, or 15 m from the speaker). Other options for experiments that include both naive and experienced individuals could be to account for species-specific responses in the design and planning of behavioural experiments, and to add human disturbance in the data analysis as an ordered factor.

In most other studies, the response most often associated with successive approaches to the same individual was a decreasing intensity of vocal response, related to habituation—a learning process in which individuals decrease or cease their responses to a stimulus after its repeated or prolonged presentations (Dong and Clayton 2009). Instead, we found that naive and experienced males demonstrated contrasting vocal behaviour during the playback phase of the experiment, i.e. naive males sung significantly more often while experienced males produced more chink and hreet calls. One of the most important functions of bird song is rival deterrence, while calls are produced in particular contexts, related to specific functions such as signalling a threat or alarm, mobbing, or expressing emotions (Catchpole and Slater 2008). Therefore, naive males, which are not familiar with playback and humans, respond to playback by singing. Such behaviour could be considered a typical reaction to playback, which has been observed in many bird species in the context of territorial defence (Bradley and Mennill 2009; Osiejuk 2014). However, the experienced individuals might recall the negative experience of capture (associated with luring by playback, capturing, and handling) and be more vigilant in a similar risky situation (Linhart et al. 2012; Harris and Haskell 2013). Additionally, playback has been reported to change daily individual activity (Mennill et al. 2002; Langham et al. 2006) and influence hormone secretion, particularly by elevating corticosterone and testosterone levels (Wingfield 1985; Wingfield et al. 2001). Furthermore, birds exposed to playback alter their vocal activity, but not always in the same way [i.e. some increase their vocal activity while others decrease it (Harris and Haskell 2013)]. Increased vocalisation requires higher energy expenditure, which could be regarded as an increase in physiological costs. In contrast, decreases in vocalisation over time could be explained by a process of habituation to playback. This type of result was previously noted during playback experiments on the Plain-tailed Wren (Pheugopedius euophrys) (Harris and Haskell 2013), and we observed a similar reaction here in the Chaffinch.

Experienced males used alarm calls instead of territorial song. Interestingly, the hreet and chink calls seem to have different functions (Marler 1956, 2004). Chink calls were observed extremely rarely during the pre-playback phase; males produced them mostly during the playback phase and less often during the post-playback phase. This suggests that chink calls could be an alarm signal related to high levels of stress, and may be produced by both males and females in various risky situations (Marler 1956, 2004). The opposite pattern was observed for the hreet calls, which were the most numerous during the post-playback phase and less frequent during the playback phase. Moreover, Chaffinch males are able to change the amplitude of hreet calls (personal observations) in a way that is similar to their manipulation of song amplitude (Brumm and Ritschard 2011). Song amplitude in Chaffinch is an aggressive signal, and playback of loud songs from a speaker elicits a stronger response from males (Brumm and Ritschard 2011). Therefore, we hypothesise that hreet calls may serve a similar function to songs, and may be used in aggressive interactions between males in order to evaluate the quality of an intruder.

Our study showed that experienced and naive males responded differently to the same playback treatment. Although the experienced birds were familiar with playback and certain humans, the main playback experiment used a different type of songs than those used to lure birds for initial capture, and the observers during the main experiment were not the same humans who had captured birds initially. It seems possible, then, that experienced birds may modify their behaviour and respond differently than naive males not only to playback but also in interactions with other males. Alternatively, experienced males may be able to distinguish between recorded songs and a natural male’s song, or they may recognise and associate the two types of threat—human presence and playback of song—and modify their behaviour only when both threats are present. The question of which mechanism is at work in Chaffinches is still open, as it is for many other bird species. In any event, future studies should include preliminary tests of the effect of birds’ previous experience of capture by humans on their subsequent responses to playback. If these tests reveal strong biases in the response of experienced birds to playback, alternative capture methods should be considered (such as capture at the nest), or subsequent playback experiments should target only inexperienced birds.

In conclusion, our study showed that Chaffinch males’ previous experiences of humans and song playback modify their behaviour during a subsequent playback experiment, at least in the short term. Experienced males modified their vocal behaviour by responding predominantly with alarm calls, while naive males tended to use song in response to a simulated intrusion within a territory. We hypothesise that experienced males’ memory of capture and their awareness of the circumstances surrounding that negative experience cause a high level of stress and anxiety, which the males may express using calls instead of songs. This change of playback-induced expression of behaviour needs more examination in a physiological context, especially regarding hormone-behaviour interrelationships. The differences shown here between naive and experienced males in response to playback are strong evidence that great care must be taken in planning, conducting, and analysing experiments that involve the repeated use of the same subjects.