Introduction

Dispersal is one of the most important, yet least understood, features of ecology, population biology and evolution (Wiens 2001). Despite numerous efforts (e.g. three edited volumes during the last decade: Clobert et al. 2001, 2012; Bullock et al. 2002), we are still a long way from comprehending the phenomenon, with Clobert et al. (2012) concluding that “despite this interest and although the importance of this particular life-history trait is widely recognized, it is perhaps understudied with its overall importance in spatial ecology”. This is mostly a consequence of the enormous difficulties of gathering observations on the movements of individuals through the landscape. Theoretical predictions cannot be tested because the data required to verify them are impossible to collect.

Hopefully, with the development of new tracking technologies (e.g. Koenig et al. 1996; Meyburg and Meyburg 2009; Stutchbury et al. 2009; Bächler et al. 2010), the impediment to data collection will be gradually removed. However, there seem to be additional, and conceptual, reasons hindering progress. Theoreticians tend to treat dispersal as a unique phenomenon that could be explained with a single set of rules, in the hope of a general theory of dispersal. However, achievement of this goal could prove impossible, as qualitatively different phenomena which have only one feature in common (organisms permanently changing their location at some point during their life) are combined under the same name. In a recent review, Matthysen (2012) underlined this multitude of causes and processes of dispersal in different groups of organisms. It seems that, even for a single group of organisms (birds) using one habitat type (forest), it would be impossible to formulate a singular explanation. Desrochers et al. (1999) concluded their review by saying “the taxonomic variety of response to forest fragmentation is bewildering. It would thus appear futile to search for species-independent theory of fragmentation, even with forest specialists only”.

Therefore, it seems more profitable to recognise these qualitative differences at the outset and to propose more specific explanations of dispersal in these different groups, including birds. Evolutionary history of the organisms concerned should always be considered. For instance, it is crucial to know whether living in a fragmented landscape is deeply embedded in a species’ evolutionary past, and whether it originated in naturally patchy landscapes, or if this is a novel situation in which they have to cope for the first time. We should expect to detect well-developed mechanisms for, e.g., crossing hostile habitats only in the former group, while these could be largely missing in the latter, which had no previous need to evolve them. Another qualitative difference to consider is the compulsion to disperse, and whether the organism is an obligatory disperser (i.e. cannot complete its life cycle without successfully dispersing), or a facultative one (dispersal is possible but not essential, and the life cycle could be completed without moving from the birthplace). For obligatory dispersers, the reasons to disperse are twofold (reviewed in Southwood 1962): (1) organisms have to switch habitats during their life cycle, e.g. amphibians, numerous holometabolous insects or host-switching parasites, and (2) organisms live in ephemeral habitats, e.g. places which are useable for periods shorter than their life-span, or the habitats do not remain suitable for more than one generation. Failure to disperse in such species is strongly selected against (sterile death), thus all must have well-developed dispersal mechanisms. Southwood (1962) even considered the ability to colonise unstable or temporary habitats to be the prime factor selecting for evolution of dispersal. On the other hand, in organisms living in permanent (i.e. lasting for many generations) habitats, selection for the development of dispersal mechanisms would not be so strong. Apart from habitat permanence, its spatial extent is also critically important. Some organisms live in naturally fragmented habitats and must frequently cross inhospitable areas, while others living in extensive (i.e. much larger than the range of the organisms’ possible displacements) habitats can spend whole their lives within it.

Dispersal is considered to be costly due to increased energetic and physiological expenditures on movement, the development of locomotory structures enabling this movement, and the increased exposure to predation and starvation during the dispersal process and/or from reduced familiarity with ecological and social conditions in novel environments (Greenwood 1980; review in Bonte et al. 2012). Thus, if dispersal is costly, then site-tenacity (non-dispersal) should be advantageous wherever possible, or it should be outweighed by the benefits of dispersal. While site-tenacity is a non-option in all obligatory dispersers, it could be favoured in organisms inhabiting long-lasting and spatially extensive habitats. Frequent evolution of flightlessness in insects in forests, which tend to be both persistent and spatially extensive habitats (Southwood 1962; Roff 1994; Hunter 1995), lends some support to this idea.

There is an enormous body of theoretical and modelling work devoted to predicting and explaining dispersal of organisms living in patchy, heterogeneous landscapes (summarised in Clobert et al. 2001, 2012; Bullock et al. 2002), yet there are few predictions of dispersal patterns in organisms living in stable, spatially extensive and largely homogenous landscapes, such as large forests. Should such organisms disperse at all? Hamilton and May (1977) answered affirmatively, suggesting that “substantial dispersal is to be expected when the habitat is uniform, constant, and occupied completely”. They argued that offspring should leave natal patches to avoid competition with their parents and siblings, and, as natal patches are already occupied, the only way to find a vacant home-range is to look elsewhere. Remaining in the same place and waiting for parental death is an alternative solution, but settling away from their kin also avoids inbreeding, as offspring would be less likely to mate with close relatives (Greenwood 1980). Hamilton and May (1977) do not provide any predictions of how far, or in what direction, an organism should disperse, nor of any effects of social status on dispersal. One can propose, though, that, in organisms with overlapping generations and simultaneously producing multiple offspring, all young should disperse to avoid competition with surviving parents, but settling just outside their parents’ ranges should suffice. However, this would not prevent competition among siblings, and, to minimise this, young should (1) disperse independently in different directions, and (2) settle at a distance that efficiently minimises the probability of meeting a sibling. As the probability of such an occurrence declines very quickly with distance, moving only a couple of home ranges distant from the natal patch would suffice. As such, short-distance dispersal should prevail and be achieved without leaving the natal habitat type. Therefore, in this group of organisms, we should not expect to find morphological, physiological or behavioural adaptations for long-term dispersal across inhospitable environments. Loss of flight in forest insects (see above) and “psychological flightlessness” (Diamond 1981) of numerous tropical forest birds, which are extremely reluctant to cross even narrow open spaces (Desrochers et al. 1999; review in Moore et al. 2008), indicate that, in permanent, extensive habitats, such adaptations are selected against.

Every dispersing organism has to perform its dispersal movement once, shifting between its place of birth and site of reproduction, termed natal dispersal (Greenwood 1980). Additionally, the iteroparous organisms have to decide what to do between consecutive breeding attempts, whether to stay in the same place or to move again, representing breeding dispersal (Greenwood 1980). It is generally assumed that an organism living in a persistent habitat where it successfully reproduced should be philopatric, staying in the same place for subsequent breeding attempts (reviews in Newton 1979; Greenwood 1980; Greenwood and Harvey 1982; Wesołowski 2006). Such individuals should be more prone to dispersal after breeding failure, but, if conditions are homogenous over large areas, there would be no clear benefits of moving, as staying or departing would have similar fitness effects (e.g. Wesołowski 2006). As an individual leaving a patch would have to trade all advantages of fidelity (familiarity, social dominance) for all costs of finding a new place and becoming established, it would probably not be worthwhile. Thus, it is expected that, in such stable, homogenous environments, breeding dispersal would be very limited and site tenacity should prevail.

To explore these ideas, I use observations of natal and breeding dispersal of Marsh Tits Poecile palustris (a small passerine weighing 10–11 g). Previous studies carried out in fragmented woods (Berndt and Winkel 1987; Nilsson 1989, Amann 1997; Markovets 2001a; Broughton et al. 2010) suggest that this species is a specialist of mature forests, and seems to have short dispersal distances. Here, I use observations on a population living in the middle of large (ca. 1,500 km2) expanse of the Białowieża Forest (Polish/Belarussian border), where the last remnants of the primeval forests of temperate Europe are protected (Tomiałojć et al.1984; Faliński 1986; Wesołowski 2007). Observations were carried out in the strictly protected part of the Białowieża National Park (hereafter BNP). Free of direct human interference, the resident, sedentary Marsh Tits breed in non-excavated holes (Wesołowski 1989, 1996, 1999, 2001), which are superabundant (Wesołowski 2001). The majority of holes persist until the following season (Wesołowski 1995, 2001) and they are often reused (Wesołowski 2006). Marsh Tits in BNP (and elsewhere) form permanent pairs breeding in exclusive territories of ca. 5 ha (Amann 1997; Broughton et al. 2006; own unpublished data), and rear one brood per year (Wesołowski 1998, 2000, 2002).

Furthermore, I compare the BNP results with observations of Marsh Tit dispersal in fragmented landscapes (for references, see above) to examine the effect of fragmentation on dispersal in this species. I conclude with some remarks on the evolution of dispersal in long-lasting (permanent), spatially extensive habitats.

Methods

Study area

The Białowieża Forest complex is situated in the middle of the European plain, at the Polish/Belarusian border (co-ordinates of Białowieża village: 52°41′N, 23°52′E). The western part of the forest (613 km2, ca. 45 % of the area) lies in Poland, and represents a remnant of the vast lowland forests that once covered large parts of temperate Europe. Its present unique features result from its considerable size and an exceptionally good state of preservation (Tomiałojć and Wesołowski 1990; Wesołowski 2005, 2007). The majority of the tree stands in the Polish part are now under management, but a 47.5-km2 block of the best preserved primeval old-growth stands, situated in the centre of the forest (Fig. 1), has been retained within the strictly protected part of BNP.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Distribution of major habitat types and permanent study plots (K, C, W and M) in the strictly protected part of the Białowieża National Park (BNP), and the location of BNP within the Białowieża Forest (after Wesołowski et al. 2002, modified). Natal dispersal directions and distances of Marsh Tits Poecile palustris hatched in the study plots but recorded outside are also shown

The preserved primeval stands are multi-storey, mixed-species, and uneven-aged. They contain many veteran trees (the tallest Norway spruce Picea abies can reach 50 m, and several other species reach 42–45 m). The girth at breast height of the thickest trees ranges from ca. 410 cm (Norway maples Acer platanoides and aspens Populus tremula) to ca. 740 cm (pedunculate oak Quercus robur; Niechoda and Korbel 2011). They also contain large amounts of standing dead timber and fallen trees (20–25 % of total wood volume; Bobiec 2002). For more information and photos, see Tomiałojć and Wesołowski (1990, 2004), Wesołowski (2007) and Wesołowski et al. (2010).

Data on Marsh Tit dispersal were gathered in four sample plots of 33–54 ha within the BNP, spaced 1–2 km apart, and covering a total area of ca. 185 ha (Fig. 1). Plots C and M were situated in the forest interior, whereas Plots W and K bordered open, formerly cultivated, areas to the south and/or west (Fig. 1). These edges were not sharp, however, as the plots were surrounded by a dense belt of young trees. Three plots (C, M, and W) were situated in oak–lime–hornbeam stands composed mostly of hornbeam Carpinus betulus, lime Tilia cordata, pedunculate oak, spruce, and continental maple. The fourth plot (K) was located in a swampy riverine forest made up mainly by alder Alnus glutinosa, ash Fraxinus excelsior, and spruce. It also contained some drier hornbeam-covered ‘islands’ along the edges (for detailed descriptions, see Wesołowski et al. 2002, 2006,; Tomiałojć and Wesołowski 1990, 2005). No artificial food nor nest boxes were available, and birds bred exclusively in natural tree holes (Wesołowski 1996, 2001).

Field observations

Observations were carried out in 1993–2013. Every spring intensive searches for nests, aimed at finding all breeding holes, were made in the four plots (Wesołowski 2002). To establish the number and distribution of Marsh Tit breeding pairs in the plots, birds were followed and their movements noted on field maps just before the onset of breeding. Many of the birds were colour-ringed (see below), which facilitated mapping work and nest searches. All marked birds were carefully searched for in the plots (they usually stayed in the same territory as in the previous year or moved to a neighbouring territory; Wesołowski 2006). During these searches, we also recorded the presence of any birds wearing only standard metal rings, which were fitted to nestlings in previous years (see below). Additionally, ringed Marsh Tits were casually searched for in all study plots used for bird censusing (Wesołowski et al. 2010) and in other parts of the Białowieża Forest.

To gather data on the fate of nests, the breeding holes were checked regularly, mostly from the ground. The observation distance varied with the situation of hole and the parents’ behaviour, in most cases being within ca. 15–50 m. The frequency of nest visits depended on the stage of the nesting cycle (see below). After incubation had been recorded, the nest was inspected to count eggs and to predict the presumable hatching date. Lower holes were inspected from a ladder, the higher ones by climbing. A flashlight bulb fixed to a flexible wire was used to light the cavity interiors. In some holes, additional usage of a small mirror on a bendable handle was necessary. It was usually possible to find holes of all pairs breeding within the plots, as well as the majority of those nesting in their close vicinity. The majority of them were accessible, and over 90 % of holes could be directly inspected. After hatching was recorded, holes with young were visited again and the age of nestlings was established following the criteria of Winkel (1970). This allowed for predicting dates of ringing of nestlings and adults, as well as fledging dates. Unringed adults or birds wearing only metal rings were caught in door-traps or mist-nets placed at the hole entrance, and colour-ringed. This was done only in calm weather, at holes with 10- to 14-day-old young. The majority of territorial birds (up to 90 % in some years) were ringed by the end of the season. Young were extracted from holes, using a “lasso” made of a thick guitar string attached to a strong wire, and ringed when 13–14 days old.

Around the expected time of fledging, the holes were observed from a distance every ca. 24 h, up to the day on which no parents were observed bringing food. If, on the previous day, young were at least 18 days old (the youngest age of fledging of undisturbed broods; Wesołowski 2000) and no signs of attempted robbing by predators were detectable, the nest was considered to be successful. If no feeding was observed at a hole containing young about to fledge (16–17 days old), we searched for parent birds to check whether they were collecting food for prematurely fledged young. If they were, the nest was classified as successful. All other cases of premature cessation of parental activity (no signs of parent presence during an hour observation session) were treated as nest failure. As such, derived fledging dates were precise to ±1 day.

Data analysis

Each year, the location of breeding holes was marked on detailed maps of the plots in reference to permanent marks forming a 50 × 50 m grid. The location of holes was made with ±5 m precision. Distances between the location of holes of the same bird in consecutive years were measured on the maps (rounded to 10 m). The maximum dispersal distance detectable within a single plot ranged from 950 (plot K) to 1,250 m (plot M), the maximum detectable dispersal distance between the neighbouring plots was from 1.9 km (plots K–W, Fig. 1) to 3.1 km (plots C–M). The largest dispersal distance detectable within the study areas (plots K–M, Fig. 1) amounted to 5.5 km and dispersal falling within 95 % of the linear inter- and intra-plot distances within this limit could be measured. Only birds dispersing between 3.6 and 3.9 km would fall outside of all plots, although such distances could still be detected from the casual searches.

Natal dispersal amounted to the distance between the position of the birth hole of a bird and the breeding hole in the following year. Breeding dispersal was expressed as the distance between the breeding holes of the same bird in successive years.

To account for inter-year differences in the timing of breeding, we calculated relative fledging dates, i.e. differences between an absolute fledging date and the median fledging dates in the respective seasons. The calculated fledging dates were then used to check whether the timing of fledging within a season influenced the natal dispersal distance.

Family size refers to the number of nestlings at ringing (13–14 days old), as long as no traces of nestling mortality (attempted nest robbing, dead young in nest after fledging) past that age were detectable. This was true in almost all (72/74) broods from which the analysed birds originated.

Population density was expressed as a summed density (individuals/10 ha) of adult and juvenile Marsh Tits at the end of the breeding season, across the whole study area. As all territorial birds were paired, adult density equalled the density of territories at the onset of spring multiplied by two. The juvenile density was calculated as territory density × proportion of successful broods × family size. As complete nest histories were largely available, there was no need to apply Mayfield corrections, as the proportion of successful broods was calculated directly from the field data. Because year-to-year changes in numbers and breeding performance of Marsh Tit in individual plots were highly synchronous (Wesołowski 1998; Wesołowski et al. 2006, 2010), a single density index was calculated for each year. The values calculated this way are somewhat exaggerated as adult mortality during the breeding season (rather low, Wesołowski and Rowiński 2006; own unpublished data), nor the juvenile mortality between fledgling and gaining independence were not accounted for. There are no data from BNP on the latter aspect, but results from Sweden (Nilsson and Smith 1985) suggest that it could also be low.

Statistics

Nonparametric tests were generally used, except a GLM analysis of effects of fledging date, family size and population density on the natal dispersal distance. All statistical analyses followed formulae in Statistica 10.0 (StatSoft 2011). All probability values shown are two-tailed.

Results

Natal dispersal

Distances between the place of birth and breeding locality were recorded in 29 Marsh Tit females and 45 males born in 1993–2012. For 73 % of birds, these were their first breeding places; the other birds were identified only when breeding for their second or subsequent time. The inclusion of the latter group was justified, because the breeding dispersal distances, in comparison with the natal dispersal movements, were very short (see below). This introduced some “noise” in the data, yet it allowed for maximisation of the sample size.

The birds dispersed from the their birth places in different directions; the longest recorded movement was of a bird hatched in plot K and caught in the autumn of the same year at the northern edge of the Białowieża Forest, 21.8 km NNE from its birth place (Fig. 1; not included in further analyses). Most often, birds were found breeding within their natal plots (Table 1), but those hatched in one plot could be found breeding in any other (Table 1). Exchange of young occurred more often between neighbouring plots than between more distant ones (cf. Table 1; Fig. 1). Fledglings from the same brood which were subsequently located after settling had dispersed in opposite directions (Table 2), though one case of sister and brother dispersing in the same direction and forming an incestuous pair was also recorded (their clutch and brood size were standard for that season, but the brood was partially reduced because of nest soaking after rain). No bird remained in its natal territory, and in all cases in which parents and offspring were known to survive (n = 16 females, 24 males), the no inter-generational pairings were observed.

Table 1 Pattern of natal dispersal of Marsh Tits Poecile palustris in BNP
Table 2 Natal dispersal of individual Marsh Tit siblings from the same nest

The natal dispersal distances of males (median = 570 m, Q 25–75 % = 390–1,720 m, Fig. 2) were three times shorter than those of the females (median = 1,720 m, Q 25–75 % = 600–2,770 m). These differences were significant: Mann–Whitney test, Z = 3.52, P < 0.0005. Apart from this intersexual difference, the dispersal distances were not related to any of the measured factors (Table 3); early- and late-fledged young, and young from the smaller and larger broods travelled similar distances (Fig. 3). Variation in population density did not produce any visible effect on the dispersal distances of individual birds (Fig. 3). Also, population density was not related to the annual mean dispersal distance in females (n = 13, r s = 0.17, P = 0.57) nor in males (n = 14, r s = −0.17, P = 0.54).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Frequency distribution of Marsh Tit natal dispersal distances in BNP in females (n = 29) and males (n = 45)

Table 3 Relationship between relative fledging date, relative brood size, and population density and natal dispersal distances of female and male Marsh Tits in BNP
Fig. 3
figure 3

Distribution of female and male Marsh Tit natal dispersal distances in BNP in relation to relative fledging dates (upper), relative family size (middle) and population density (lower). Details of how the values of independent variables were calculated are given in "Methods"

Breeding dispersal

From the first breeding onwards, adult birds remained in the same area and no dispersal between study plots was observed. In consecutive years, the birds either used the nest hole from the previous year (distance = 0 m) or moved to a new hole over a distance of up 520 m (one male, Fig. 4). Generally, the distances moved between years were much shorter than this, with a median of 100 m (Q 25–75 % = 30–140 m, n = 12) in females and 70 m (Q 25–75 % = 40–140 m, n = 25) in males. The distances moved did not differ substantially between the sexes (Mann–Whitney test, Z adjusted = 0.77, P = 0.44). Additionally, long-lived individuals (breeding for 5–9 years, n = 6) remained in very restricted areas, with the maximum distance between nest holes ranging over only 150–240 m. The breeding dispersal distances were an order of magnitude shorter than those of natal dispersal (cf. Figs. 2, 4). This difference was highly significant, both in females (Wilcoxon Matched Pairs test, Z = 3.06, P = 0.0022) and in males (Z = 4.37, P < 0.0001).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Frequency distribution of Marsh Tit breeding dispersal distances in BNP in females (n = 31 movements of 12 individuals) and in males (n = 53 movements of 25 individuals)

Discussion

Natal dispersal

This study was conducted within a set of permanent plots within an extensive habitat matrix. As such, birds settling within the plots had a 100 % probability of being detected, while birds settling outside of the plots had only a very small chance of being found. Moreover, the maximum dispersal distance which could be recorded within the study areas amounted to 5.5 km, and birds moving any further had only a remote chance of being detected. Therefore, the distribution of natal dispersal distances and dispersal directions found in this study is not an unbiased sample. Constraints on the distribution of dispersal distances constitutes a common problem for studies within limited areas, as they are usually too restricted to detect long-distance dispersal (Barrowclough 1978; Baker et al. 1995; Koenig et al. 1996).

However, the ringing recoveries from other Marsh Tit populations (which are unaffected by this problem) show that 64 % of birds were found up to 6 km from the birth places, 87 % up to 13 km, and all within 100 km (Rost 1988). In Britain, 85 % of 108 recoveries were within 5 km (Sellers 1984), with only three above 20 km (review in Cramp and Perrins 1993). There is just one reliable observation of a Marsh Tit moving more than 100 km: a bird was observed in Scotland, at least 135 km from the nearest known breeding population (Hamlin 2008).

Therefore, local studies of dispersal (summarised in Table 4), in which movements of up to 5–7 km could be detected, probably encompassed the majority of dispersal events in local populations. As the detection range of these studies was similar to that in BNP, the results could be cautiously compared across the sites. However, within-area comparisons in the current study are not compromised, as all classes of Marsh Tit in BNP were equally affected by the constraints of the study design. The natal dispersal distances recorded in BNP were within the range of those recorded elsewhere, except for the Russian study, where Marsh Tits dispersed over much greater distances (Table 4). The latter were probably due to specific structural features of the local landscape, as birds there bred in isolated deciduous patches embedded in a very narrow belt of coniferous forest stretching along the Curonian Spit (Bardin et al. 1992; Markovets 2001a, b). Females in BNP dispersed greater distances than males, as observed in the majority of other areas studied (Table 4), although this difference was most pronounced in BNP. As such, females were much less likely than the males to settle within the study plots. This probably accounted for the lower proportion of females (39 %) among the detected recruits, despite their equal proportion at fledging (Czyż et al. 2012). Such female-biased dispersal seems characteristic for many birds (reviews in Greenwood 1980; Clarke et al. 1997; Paradis et al. 1998). For male Marsh Tits, which have to establish a territory, they were probably more pressed to do so than females as soon as possible after departure from the natal territory. The females, being less constrained, could possibly spend more time roaming over larger areas to select both a site and a mate (Nilsson 1989; Amann 1997).

Table 4 Sexual differences in natal dispersal distances of Marsh Tits in different study areas

All young Marsh Tits left their natal territories in BNP and in other areas (Berndt and Winkel 1987; Nilsson 1989; Bardin et al. 1992; Amann 1997; Broughton et al. 2010). Additionally, observations from other areas show that young Marsh Tits depart their natal areas immediately after becoming independent, and leave without any aggression from parents or siblings (Nilsson and Smith 1985; Amann 1997). Thus, It appears that emigration from the birth place constitutes a compulsory phase in the Marsh Tit life cycle, and that the decision to leave per se is independent of local environmental and social factors. The latter, though, could still have some impact on the distances travelled. The idea that dispersal distances should change with varying densities of conspecifics is popular among theoreticians, yet empirical support for this notion is rather scant. In a recent critical review, Matthysen (2005) found that, despite the frequently expressed view of the role of increasing density in stimulating dispersal, signs of such a relationship were detected only in 7 of 21 avian studies testing this idea. Among the results he questioned was a study by Nilsson (1989), showing that dispersal distances in Swedish Marsh Tits increased when population density increased. Matthysen (2005) argued that this could be a spurious result because the number of years studied was very small (only four), and one particular year appeared to greatly influence the outcome. Results from the current study in BNP, based on 14 years of observations during which the post-fledging density of Marsh Tits varied over a much larger range than in Sweden, did not show any relationship between density and dispersal distance. Furthermore, changes in density within a season (the first young of the season face 2–3 times lower population density than the last ones hatched), or variation in family size, did not influence the dispersal distances in BNP, in contrast to the Swedish study (Nilsson 1989). Extending dispersal distances in response to increased local crowding could only help where there was some density gradient, and that by moving further away the birds would be able to find less crowded areas. This was not the case, though, in the largely homogenous BNP, where the whole area was saturated with birds and the population density and breeding productivity varied across all areas in parallel (see below).Therefore, all locations within a bird’s reach were equally crowded, and in such conditions the extension of dispersal distances would probably not offer any benefit.

The costs of dispersal movements themselves often represent the largest costs of the emigration–transfer–settlement sequence (reviewed in Bonte et al. 2012). These appeared negligible in BNP, as Marsh Tits moved through suitable habitat and an entire dispersal distance could be covered in a single flight of a few minutes in duration, and birds probably covered greater distances while foraging within a territory during a single day. Therefore, the costs of transfer could not account for the dispersal patterns found in BNP. As the costs were negligible, some individual variation in dispersal propensity could have been tolerated, dispersing further than the necessary minimum would not be selected against. However, though relatively unimportant from the individual perspective, and relatively infrequent, the displacements over tens of kilometres could have profound population effects. The regular exchange of individuals (and genes) over long distances would prevent local differentiation, and would result in ecologically and genetically unified populations inhabiting extensive areas (review in Williamson 2002). The role of such long-distance dispersers could be even greater in Marsh Tits living in highly fragmented woods (see below).

It was impossible to check whether dispersal distance affected Marsh Tit breeding performance in BNP, as found in some other species (Pärt 1991; Pasinelli et al. 2004), because only effective dispersal (Greenwood 1980) was recorded in the current study, and adults were caught and identified only when feeding large young. The sample was therefore biased towards successful individuals, which had managed to settle, survive until the breeding season, and whose broods had not been destroyed. However, as Marsh Tits in all the study plots commenced breeding at the same time (Wesołowski 1998), laid a similar number of eggs (Wesołowski 2000), and were equally successful in production of young (Wesołowski 2002), it would not matter whether they dispersed for a short distance and settled in the natal plot or moved to some distant place. The two-way exchange of birds between neighbouring plots in BNP also indicated that birds perceived all areas as equally attractive, and, in such conditions, dispersal distance should not affect breeding performance.

Breeding dispersal

After their first breeding, Marsh Tits in BNP remained in the same area of forest for life. Although they often shifted location of the breeding holes between years (Wesołowski 2006), the median distance between consecutive holes was very short (70–100 m in both sexes), and the maximum distance between nest holes used by the same birds in 5–9 breeding years only varied between 150 and 240 m. Breeding birds were similarly site-tenacious in other studies: in Lower Saxony, Germany, >95 % bred within <300 m of the previous year’s nestbox (Berndt and Winkel 1987); in Switzerland, 87 % of birds remained in the same or neighbouring territory in subsequent years (Amann 2003); and in England, 95 % of birds remained in the same territory between years (Broughton et al. 2010). Reports from other areas indicate the same pattern (Rost 1988; Markovets 2001a, b). The rare cases in which greater territory shifts are observed are mostly due to the death of a mate, disappearance of neighbouring territory owners, or (rarely) divorces (Amann 2003; Broughton et al. 2010). It appears, thus, that such site faithfulness is a species-specific trait of the Marsh Tit. As an extreme example of this behaviour, Rost (1988) described an individual spending up to 11 years in the same forest patch, of just few hundred metres across, a distance which could be flown in a matter of seconds. Such extreme site tenacity could only evolve in very predictable, long-lasting habitats where suitability remained adequate over many years. The old-growth forest habitats used by Marsh Tits in BNP had such characteristics, and changed very slowly (Wesołowski et al. 2010), so from the perspective of a Marsh Tit’s lifetime (a decade or less), the habitat structure remained largely unchanged.

Ultimate causes of Marsh Tit dispersal

By leaving their natal patches, young birds can achieve twofold benefits. Firstly, they avoid competition with their parents and siblings (Hamilton and May 1977) and, secondly, by settling away from their kin, the young would also avoid incestuous breeding (Greenwood 1980). Marsh tit siblings in BNP probably dispersed independently of one another, in disparate directions, as was surmised in Switzerland (Amann 1997). However, in Lower Saxony, Berndt and Winkel (1987) observed that siblings (four cases) dispersed in similar directions, although, as in other areas (see above), they settled at different distances. Such a pattern of dispersal would, to a large extent, prevent incestuous mating with siblings. Furthermore, because of the extreme site fidelity of adults (see above), this automatically excluded the possibility of incestuous mating between parents and their offspring (no such cases observed in BNP). The Marsh Tit results are thus consistent with data for other forest species (Greenwood et al. 1978; Schiegg et al. 2006; Szulkin and Sheldon 2008), which suggest that dispersal could serve as a primary mechanism of inbreeding avoidance. It remains to be seen, though, whether inbreeding avoidance has played a major role in shaping the evolution of dispersal in Marsh Tits, or whether it is simply an advantageous by-product of other selective pressures, such as avoidance of intra-specific competition between kin (Nilsson 1989). As the natal patch is already occupied (by parents and fledglings), the only way for a juvenile bird to find a vacant place to settle is to search elsewhere. Even in a homogenous, saturated habitat, such opportunities can always occur, due to the death of adults. In this case, a bird would have to compete for a free slot but it would be in competition with strangers and not outcompeting relatives. The alternative solution, of staying on the natal territory, would be far less beneficial as it would denote resignation from breeding and queuing for a breeding opportunity. This is known to occur in situations where dispersal is for some reasons restricted (e.g. by habitat availability, islands) and all suitable patches are permanently occupied. Such conditions frequently led to evolution of co-operative breeding systems (reviews in Brown 1987; Ligon 1999).

Fragmentation

Marsh Tits in BNP still live in extensive, relatively homogenous forest, where most birds can presumably spend their whole lives without ever leaving suitable habitat. These old growth deciduous stands also provide very stable conditions, which makes possible the extreme site tenacity of breeding adults. Birds adapted to such conditions have probably not evolved mechanisms for long-distance dispersal across inhospitable landscapes, so how might they respond to habitat fragmentation and the appearance of dispersal barriers? Other studies of Marsh Tit dispersal carried out in such fragmented forests (see above) allow for comparisons. These Marsh Tits have not changed their basic pattern of dispersal behaviour: all young leave their natal patch, natal dispersal is usually the greatest distance moved during a lifetime, there is extreme site tenacity from the first breeding onwards, and natal dispersal distances are broadly similar (Table 4). This is to be expected, as, depending on how recent and extensive was the habitat disruption, the reaction could differ. If fragmentation was a very recent phenomenon, then birds would probably just avoid venturing into open areas (Desrochers et al. 1999; Lens et al. 1999; reviews in Desrochers and Bélisle 2007; Moore et al. 2008) which would result in shorter dispersal distances. However, if habitat becomes extremely patchy, then birds have to increase dispersal distances to avoid extinction. Yet, as the dispersal distances can only be measured for successful individuals which managed to settle somewhere, the distribution of dispersal distances does not provide any information on dispersal failures, i.e. the proportion of young which fail to settle anywhere. This effect could be quite substantial, though. The review of Desrochers and Bélisle (2007) show that, of all parid species analysed, Marsh Tits are the most sensitive to fragmentation, being first to disappear from smaller, more isolated woodlots situated in more open landscapes. These findings are confirmed by intensive studies in a highly fragmented landscape in England (Alderman et al. 2011; Broughton 2012). Marsh Tits are reluctant to cross open areas, with gaps greater than around 100 m beginning to represent significant barriers to movement (Ludescher 1973; Broughton and Hinsley 2014). The presence of unoccupied patches and territories occupied by unpaired males observed in England (a phenomenon nonexistent in BNP; T. Wesołowski, unpublished) indicates that settlement failure of longer-dispersing females could be a problem for that Marsh Tit population (Dale 2001; Broughton 2012). Whereas birds in BNP can disperse in any direction while remaining in suitable habitat and being able to find conspecifics everywhere, the birds in the highly fragmented English woodlands have to move further between patches in order to encounter the same number of potential territories where they could settle, often crossing hostile landscapes, and may never find a suitable patch or a mate. Matthysen and Currie (1996) observed identical symptoms of reduced disperser success in another highly sedentary forest species, the Eurasian Nuthatch Sitta europaea. Such problems with natal dispersal seem to be a major factor in the widespread decline of Marsh Tits in Britain (Broughton and Hinsley 2014).

Concluding remarks

Studies of dispersal continue to be a hot topic, and the phenomenon continues to demand theoretical interest and generates much empirical work. However, these efforts concentrate largely on organisms living in fragmented, transient habitats (see "Introduction"). The dispersal of organisms living in spatially extensive, homogenous, long-lasting environments remains largely unstudied. This is very unfortunate, as this very group of organisms tends to be the one that is most detrimentally affected by fragmentation and the appearance of dispersal barriers, so is of greatest conservation concern (Hunter 1996; Macdonald and Johnson 2001; Baguette et al. 2012). Studying the dispersal behaviour of this group of organisms is also of enormous value for basic science, as it is necessary for understanding the evolution of life histories, social behaviour, population genetics or even large-scale biogeographic patterns. Whereas birds in the temperate zone are rarely as sedentary as Marsh Tits (e.g. Eurasian Nuthatch, Matthysen and Currie 1996 or Crested Tit Lophophanes cristatus, Lens and Dhondt 1994; Bardin 2011), this habit is much more widespread in the tropics (review in Stutchbury and Morton 2001). An additional feature of such tropical habitats, apart from their temporal persistence and spatial homogeneity, is that they tend to be permanently saturated with territories which leads to scarcity of vacant, unoccupied patches in which dispersing juveniles could settle. In such conditions, dispersal becomes even more restricted, and young birds tend to stay on the natal territories and co-operative breeding often evolves (review in Brown 1987; Stutchbury and Morton 2001). Dispersal constraints could even be a significant contributor to the origin of the enormous diversification of tropical forest avifaunas (Salisbury et al. 2012). The problems of limited dispersal and the inability to cross barriers are not restricted to birds, as they are faced by numerous other organisms living in extensive, persistent environments (e.g. insects; van Dongen et al. 1994; Wesołowski and Rowiński 2006). A greater effort invested in studying such organisms in these environments would be productive.