Skip to main content
Log in

Meaning and ostension in great ape gestural communication

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It is sometimes argued that while human gestures are produced ostensively and intentionally, great ape gestures are produced only intentionally. If true, this would make the psychological mechanisms underlying the different species’ communication fundamentally different, and ascriptions of meaning to chimpanzee gestures would be inappropriate. While the existence of different underlying mechanisms cannot be ruled out, in fact claims about difference are driven less by empirical data than by contested assumptions about the nature of ostensive communication. On some accounts, there are no reasons to doubt that great ape gestural communication is ostensive. If these accounts are correct, attributions of meaning to chimpanzee gestures would be justified.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Scott-Phillips’ analysis of meaning appeals to a clause (3) different from the one adopted here. It is now widely accepted that Grice’s original third clause, which Scott-Phillips adopts, would rule out cases intuitively described as meaningful (Neale 1992; Sperber and Wilson 1995). This clause is therefore dropped, and replaced with a different clause to protect Grice’s analysis from objections raised by philosophers including Strawson (1964) and Schiffer (1972).

References

  • Bard K, Leavens D (2014) The importance of development for comparative primatology. Annu Rev Anthropol 43:183–200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bard K, Dunbar S, Maguire-Herring V, Veira Y, Hayes K, McDonald K (2014) Gestures and social-emotional communicative development in chimpanzee infants. Am J Primatol 76(1):14–29

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-On D (2013) Origins of meaning: must we ‘go Gricean’? Mind Lang 28(3):342–375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behne T, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2005) One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Dev Sci 8:492–499

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter M, Tomasello M, Savage-Rumbaugh S (1995) Joint attention and imitative learning in children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Soc Dev 4:217–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis M (2011) The recursive mind: the origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Csibra G (2010) Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind Lang 25(2):141–168

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson D (1984) Inquiries into truth and interpretation. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (1996) Kinds of minds: the origins of consciousness. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Genty E, Breuer T, Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2009) Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): repertoire, intentionality, and possible origins. Anim Cogn 12(3):527–546

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gómez JC (1994) Mutual awareness in primate communication: a Gricean approach. In: Parker ST et al (eds) Self-awareness in animals and humans. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez JC (1996) Ostensive behaviour in great apes: the role of eye-contact. In: Russon AE et al (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez JC (2007) Pointing behaviours in apes and human infants: a balanced interpretation. Child Dev 78(3):729–734

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behaviour. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Green M (2007) Self-expression. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Grice P (1957) Meaning. Philos Rev 66(3):377–388

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice P (1969) Utterer’s meaning and intention. Philos Rev 78(2):147–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosse G, Scott-Phillips T, Tomasello M (2013) Three-year-olds hide their communicative intentions in appropriate contexts. Dev Psychol 49(11):2095–2101

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halina M, Rossano F, Tomasello M (2013) The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Anim Cogn 16(4):653–666

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare B, Tomasello M (2004) Chimpanzees are more skilful in competitive than in cooperative cognitive tasks. Anim Behav 68:571–581

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann E, Tomasello M (2006) Apes’ and children’s understanding of cooperative and competitive motives in a communicative situation. Dev Sci 9:518–529

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2011) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Anim Cogn 14(5):745–767

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2014) Curr Biol 24(14):1596–1600

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter AB, Cantaro M, Hopkins WD (2001) Differential use of vocal and gestural communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in response to the attentional status of a human (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 115(4):337–343

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leavens D, Hopkins W (1998) Intentional communication by chimpanzees: a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Dev Psychol 34(5):813–822

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leavens D, Hopkins W, Bard K (1996) Indexical and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 110(4):346–353

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leavens D, Hopkins W, Bard K (2005) Understanding the point of chimpanzee pointing: epigenesis and ecological validity. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 14(4):185–189

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leekam SR, Solomon TL, Teoh Y-S (2010) Adults’ social cues facilitate young children’s use of signs and symbols. Dev Sci 13(1):108–119

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leudar I, Costall A (2004) On the persistence of the ‘problem of other minds’ in psychology: Chomsky, Grice and ‘theory of mind’. Theor Psychol 14:601–621

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liebal K, Call J, Tomasello M (2004a) Use of gesture sequences in chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 64:377–396

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liebal K, Pika S, Call J, Tomasello M (2004b) To move or not to move: how apes adjust to the intentional states of others. Interact Stud 5(2):119–219

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyn H, Russell JL, Hopkins WD (2010) The impact of environment on the comprehension of declarative communication in apes. Psychol Sci 21(3):360–365

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore R (2013) Evidence and interpretation in great ape gestural communication. Humana-Mente 24(1):27–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore R (2014) Ontogenetic constraints on Paul Grice’s theory of communication. In: Matthews D (ed) Pragmatic development in first language acquisition. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 87–104

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore R (2015) A common intentional framework for ape and human communication. Curr Anthropol 56(1):70–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore R, Liebal K, Tomasello M (2013) Three year olds understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze. Interaction Studies 14(1):62–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore R, Mueller B, Kaminski J, Tomasello M (2015) Two-year-olds but not domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze. Dev Sci 18(2):232–242

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neale S (1992) Paul Grice and the philosophy of language. Linguist Philos 15:509–559

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Grady C, Kleisch C, Smith K, Scott-Phillips TC (2015) The ease and extent of recursive mindreading, across implicit and explicit tasks. Evol Hum Behav 36(4):313–322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Onishi K, Baillargeon R (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308(8):255–258

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli DJ, Theall LA, Reaux JE, Dunphy-Lelii S (2003) Chimpanzees spontaneously alter the location of their gestures to match the attentional orientation of others. Anim Behav 65:1–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts AI, Vick S-J, Buchanan-Smith H (2012) Usage and comprehension of manual gestures in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 84(2):459–470

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts AI, Vick S-J, Buchanan-Smith H (2013) Communicative intentions in wild chimpanzees: persistence and elaboration in gestural signalling. Anim Cog 16(2):187–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts AI, Roberts SGB, Vick S-J (2014) The repertoire and intentionality of gestural communication in wild chimpanzees. Anim Cog 17:317–336

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer SR (1972) Meaning. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips TC (2014) Speaking our minds. Palgrave MacMillan, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips TC (2015a) Nonhuman primate communication, pragmatics, and the origins of language. Curr Anthropol 56(1):56–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott-Phillips TC (2015b) Meaning in animal and human communication. Anim Cogn 18(3):801–805

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju A, Csibra G (2008) Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Curr Biol 18:668–671

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D (2000) Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In: Sperber D (ed) Metarepresentations: a multidisciplinary perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Wilson D (1995) Relevance: communication and cognition, 2nd edn. Basil Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Wilson D (2002) Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind Lang 17(1–2):3–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strawson PF (1964) Intention and convention in speech acts. Philos Rev 73(4):439–460

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2006) Why don’t apes point? In: Enfield NJ, Levinson SC (eds) Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition and interaction. Berg Publishers, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2008) Origins of human communication. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, George BL, Kruger AC, Farrar MJ, Evans A (1985) The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. J Hum Evol 14:175–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Gust D, Frost GT (1989) A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Primates 30(1):35–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Call J, Nagell K, Olguin R, Carpenter M (1994) The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates 35(2):137–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Call Warren J, Frost GT, Carpenter M, Nagell K (1997a) The ontogeny of chimpanzee gestural signals: a comparison across groups and generations. Evol Commun 1(2):223–259

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Call J, Gluckman A (1997b) Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Dev 68(6):1067–1080

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Call J, Hare B (2003) Chimpanzees understand psychological states—the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends Cogn Sci 7(4):153–156

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto S, Humle T, Tanaka M (2012) Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109(9):3588–3592

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank to Manuel Bohn, Cathy Crockford, Gema Martín Ordás, Denis Tatone and two anonymous reviewers for very helpful comments on this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Richard Moore.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Moore, R. Meaning and ostension in great ape gestural communication. Anim Cogn 19, 223–231 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0905-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0905-x

Keywords

Navigation