Skip to main content
Log in

Great apes’ strategies to map spatial relations

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated reasoning about spatial relational similarity in three great ape species: chimpanzees, bonobos, and orangutans. Apes were presented with three spatial mapping tasks in which they were required to find a reward in an array of three cups, after observing a reward being hidden in a different array of three cups. To obtain a food reward, apes needed to choose the cup that was in the same relative position (i.e., on the left) as the baited cup in the other array. The three tasks differed in the constellation of the two arrays. In Experiment 1, the arrays were placed next to each other, forming a line. In Experiment 2, the positioning of the two arrays varied each trial, being placed either one behind the other in two rows, or next to each other, forming a line. Finally, in Experiment 3, the two arrays were always positioned one behind the other in two rows, but misaligned. Results suggested that apes compared the two arrays and recognized that they were similar in some way. However, we believe that instead of mapping the left–left, middle–middle, and right–right cups from each array, they mapped the cups that shared the most similar relations to nearby landmarks (table’s visual boundaries).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barth J, Call J (2006) Tracking the displacement of objects: a series of tasks with great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus) and Young Children (Homo sapiens). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 32:239–252

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bovet D, Vauclair J (2001) Judgment of conceptual identity in monkeys. Psychon Bull Rev 8:470–475

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Call J (2001) Object permanence in Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and Children (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 115:159–171

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chen Z (2007) Learning to map: strategy discovery and strategy change in young children. Dev Psychol 43:386–403

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christie S, Gentner D (2007) Relational similarity in identity relation: the role of language. In: Vosniadou S, Kayser D, Protopapas A (eds) Proceedings of the second European cognitive science conference. Taylor & Francis, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook RG, Wasserman EA (2007) Learning and transfer of relational matching-to-sample by pigeons. Psychon Bull Rev 14:1107–1114

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Blois ST, Novak MA, Bond M (1998) Object permanence in Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). J Comp Psychol 112:137–152

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deipolyi A, Santos L, Hauser MD (2001) The role of landmarks in cotton-top tamarin spatial foraging: evidence for geometric and non-geometric features. Anim Cogn 4:99–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache JS (2004) Becoming symbol-minded. Trends Cogn Sci 8:66–70

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache JS, Brown AL (1983) Very young children’s memory for the location of objects in a large-scale environment. Child Dev 54:888–897

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache JS, de Mendoza OAP, Anderson KN (1999) Multiple factors in early symbol use: instructions, similarity, and age in understanding a symbol-referent relation. Cogn Dev 14:299–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dépy D, Fagot J, Vauclair J (1999) Processing of above: below categorical spatial relations by baboons (Papio papio). Behav Process 48:1–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diedrichsen J, Werner S, Schmidt T, Trommershäuser J (2004) Immediate spatial distortions of pointing movements induced by visual landmarks. Percept Psychophys 66:89–103

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dolins FL (2009) Captive cotton-top tamarins’ (Saguinus oedipus oedipus) use of landmarks to localize hidden food items. Am J Primatol 71:316–323

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot J, Parron C (2010) Relational matching in baboons (Papio papio) with reduced grouping requirements. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 36:184–193

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fagot J, Wasserman EA, Young ME (2001) Discriminating the relation between relations: the role of entropy in abstract conceptualization by baboons (Papio papio) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 27:316–328

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flemming TM, Beran MJ, Washburn DA (2007) Disconnect in concept learning by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): judgment of relations and relations-between-relations. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 33:55–63

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flemming TM, Beran MJ, Thompson RKR, Kleider HM, Washburn DA (2008) What meaning means for same and different: analogical reasoning in humans (Homo sapiens), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J Comp Psychol 122:176–185

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner D (2003) Why we’re so smart. In: Gentner D, Goldin-Meadow S (eds) Language in mind: advances in the study of language and thought. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 195–235

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami U (1991) Analogical reasoning: what develops? a review of research and theory. Child Dev 62:1–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami U (1995) Transitive relational mappings in three- and four-year-olds: the analogy of goldilocks and the three bears. Child Dev 66:877–892

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami U, Brown AL (1990) Melting chocolate and melting snowmen: analogical reasoning and causal relations. Cognition 35:69–95

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Halford GS, Wilson WH, Phillips S (1998) Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology. Behav Brain Sci 21:803–831

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haun DBM, Call J (2009) Great apes’ capacities to recognize relational similarity. Cognition 110:147–159

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann E, Call J, Hernández-Lloreda MV, Hare B, Tomasello M (2007) Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science 317:1360–1366

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman ML, Beran MJ (2006) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) remember the location of a hidden food item after altering their orientation to a spatial array. J Comp Psychol 120:389–393

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holland JH, Holyoak KJ, Nisbett RE, Thagard P (1986) Induction: processes of inference, learning and discovery. Bradford Books/The MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak KJ, Junn EN, Billman DO (1984) Development of analogical problem-solving skill. Child Dev 55:2042–2055

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher J, Presson CC (1979) The coding and transformation of spatial information. Cogn Psychol 11:375–394

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher J, Newcombe N, Sandberg EH (1994) The coding of spatial location in young children. Cogn Psychol 27:115–147

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy EH, Fragaszy DM (2008) Analogical reasoning in a capuchin monkey (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 122:167–175

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kroger JK, Holyoak KJ, Hummel JE (2004) Varieties of sameness: the impact of relational complexity on perceptual comparisons. Cogn Sci 28:335–358

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubo-Kawai N, Kawai N (2007) Interference effects by spatial proximity and age-related declines in spatial memory by Japanese Monkeys (Macaca fuscata): deficits in the combined use of multiple spatial cues. J Comp Psychol 121:189–197

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leech R, Mareschal D, Cooper RP (2008) Analogy as relational priming: a developmental and computational perspective on the origins of a complex cognitive skill. Behav Brain Sci 31:357–378

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein J, Gentner D (2005) Relational language and the development of relational mapping. Cogn Psychol 50:315–353

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald SE, Spetch ML, Kelly DM, Cheng K (2004) Strategies in landmark use by children, adults, and marmoset monkeys. Learn Motiv 35:322–347

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menzel CR (1996) Spontaneous use of matching visual cues during foraging by long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Psychol 110:370–376

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Penn DC, Holyoak KJ, Povinelli DJ (2008) Darwin’s mistake: explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behav Brain Sci 31:109–130

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg IM (1987) Acquisition of the same/different concept by an African grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): learning with respect to categories of color, shape and material. Anim Learn Behav 15:423–432

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potì P, Bartolommei P, Saporiti M (2005) Landmark use by Cebus apella. Int J Primatol 26:921–948

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potì P, Kanngiesser P, Saporiti M, Amiconi A, Bläsing B, Call J (2010) Searching in the Middle–Capuchins’ (Cebus apella) and Bonobos’ (Pan paniscus) behavior during a spatial search task. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 36:92–109

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn PC, Adams A, Kennedy E, Shettler L, Wasnik A (2003) Development of an abstract category representation for the spatial relation between in 6- to 10-month-old infants. Dev Psychol 39:151–163

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramscar MJA, Pain HG (1996) Can a real distinction be made between cognitive theories of analogy and categorization? In: Proceedings of the 18th annual conference of the cognitive science society, University of California, San Diego, pp 346–351

  • Rattermann MJ, Gentner D (1998) The effect of language on similarity: the use of relational labels improves young children’s performance in a mapping task. In: Holyoak KJ, Gentner D, Kokinov B (eds) Advances in analogy research: integration of theory and data from the cognitive, computational and neural sciences. New Bulgarian University, Sophia, pp 274–282

    Google Scholar 

  • Richland LE, Morrison RG, Holyoak KJ (2006) Children’s development of analogical reasoning: insights from scene analogy problems. J Exp Child Psychol 94:249–273

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simms N, Gentner D (2008) Spatial language and landmark use: can 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds find the middle? In: Love BC, McRae K, Sloutsky VM (eds) Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society. Cognitive Science Society, Austin, pp 191–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer JP, Hund AM (2003) Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall. Cognit Psychol 47:432–480

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer JP, Smith LB, Thelen E (2001) Tests of a dynamic systems account of the a-not-b error: the influence of prior experience on the spatial memory abilities of two-year-olds. Child Dev 72:1327–1346

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spinozzi G, Lubrano G, Truppa V (2004) Categorization of above and below spatial relations by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 118:403–412

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton JE, Olthof A, Roberts WA (2000) Landmark use by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Anim Learn Behav 28:28–42

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thibaut J-P, French R, Vezneva M (2008) Analogy-making in children: the importance of processing constraints, 30th annual meeting of the cognitive science society, Washington, DC

  • Thompson RKR, Oden DL, Boysen ST (1997) Language-naive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) judge relations between relations in a conceptual matching-to-sample task. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 23:31–43

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2003) Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Uttal DH, Sandstrom LB, Newcombe NS (2006) One hidden object, two spatial codes: young children’s use of relational and vector coding. J Cogn Dev 7:503–525

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vonk J (2003) Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) and Orangutan (Pongo abelii) understanding of first- and second-order relations. Anim Cogn 6:77–86

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright AA, Katz JS (2006) Mechanisms of same/different concept learning in primates and avians. Behav Process 72:234–254

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD) through a grant to the first author. We thank James Close for providing helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript as well as for improving the English of the manuscript. The reported experiments comply with all laws of the country in which they were performed.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alenka Hribar.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hribar, A., Haun, D. & Call, J. Great apes’ strategies to map spatial relations. Anim Cogn 14, 511–523 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0385-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0385-6

Keywords

Navigation