Skip to main content
Log in

Why do dogs (Canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many argue that dogs show unique susceptibility to human communicative signals that make them suitable for being engaged in complex co-operation with humans. It has also been revealed that socially provided information is particularly effective in influencing the behaviour of dogs even when the human’s action demonstration conveys inefficient or mistaken solution of task. It is unclear, however, how the communicative nature of the demonstration context and the presence of the human demonstrator affect the dogs’ object-choice behaviour in observational learning situations. In order to unfold the effects of these factors, 76 adult pet dogs could observe a communicative or a non-communicative demonstration in which the human retrieved a tennis ball from under an opaque container while manipulating another distant and obviously empty (transparent) one. Subjects were then allowed to choose either in the presence of the demonstrator or after she left the room. Results showed a significant main effect of the demonstration context (presence or absence of the human’s communicative signals), and we also found some evidence for the response-modifying effect of the presence of the human demonstrator during the dogs’ choice. That is, dogs predominantly chose the baited container, but if the demonstration context was communicative and the human was present during the dogs’ choice, subjects’ tendency to select the baited container has been reduced. In agreement with the studies showing sensitivity to human’s communicative signals in dogs, these findings point to a special form of social influence in observational learning situations when it comes to learning about causally opaque and less efficient (compared to what comes natural to the dog) action demonstrations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bekkering H, Wohlschlager A, Gattis M (2000) Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Q J Exp Psychol A 53:153–164

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Riedel J, Call J, Tomasello M (2006) Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog–causal ape. J Comp Psychol 120(1):38–47

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M (2007) Encultured chimpanzees imitate rationally. Dev Sci 10(4):31–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M (2008) Rational tool use and tool choice in human infants and great apes. Child Dev 79:609–626

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter M, Akhtar N, Tomasello M (1998) Fourteen- to 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behav Dev 21:315–330

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter M, Call J, Tomasello M (2002) Understanding ‘prior intentions’ enables 2-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. Child Dev 73:1431–1441

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cleveland A, Striano T (2007) The effects of joint attention on object processing in 4- and 9-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev 30:499–504

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra G, Gergely Gy (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn Sci 13:144–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis A, Bargh JA (2001) The perception-behavior expressway: automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 33:1–40

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erdőhegyi Á, Topál J, Virányi Zs, Miklósi Á (2007) Dog-logic: the restricted use of inferential reasoning in a two-way choice task. Anim Behav 74:725–737

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gácsi M, Győri B, Miklósi Á, Virányi Zs, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Csányi V (2005) Species-specific differences and similarities in the behaviour of hand raised dog and wolf puppies in social situations with humans. Dev Psychobiol 47:111–122

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gácsi M, McGreevy P, Kara E, Miklósi Á (2009) Effects of selection for cooperation and attention in dogs. Behav Brain Funct 5:31

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G, Csibra G (2006) Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In: Levenson S, Enfield N (eds) Roots of human sociality: culture, cognition and human interaction. Berg Publishers, Oxford, pp 229–255

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G, Bekkering H, Király I (2002) Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature 415:755

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely G, Egyed K, Király I (2007) On pedagogy. Dev Sci 10:139–146

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossmann T, Johnson MH, Lloyd-Fox S, Blasi A, Deligianni F, Elwell C, Csibra G (2008) Early cortical specialization for face-to-face communication in human infants. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 275:2803

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare B, Tomasello M (2005) The emotional reactivity hypothesis and cognitive evolution. Trends Cogn Sci 9:464–465

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horner V, Whiten A (2005) Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Anim Cogn 8:164–181

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski J, Tempelmann S, Call J, Tomasello M (2009) Domestic dogs comprehend human communication with iconic signs. Dev Sci 12:831–837

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Király I (2009) The effect of the model’s presence and of negative evidence on infants’ selective imitation. J Exp Child Psychol 102:14–25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN (1988) Infant imitation after 1-week delay: long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Dev Psychol 24:470–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Gácsi M, Virányi Zs, Csányi V (2003) A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans but dogs do. Curr Biol 13:763–766

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller HC, Rayburn-Reeves R, Zentall TR (2009) Imitation, emulation by dogs using a bidirectional control procedure. Behav Processes 80:109–114

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moll H, Tomasello M (2007) How 14-and 18-month-olds know what others have experienced? Dev Psychol 43:309–317

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moll H, Richter N, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2008) Fourteen-month-olds know what “We” have shared in a special way. Infancy 13:90–101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen M (2006) Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. Dev Psychol 42(3):555–565

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pongrácz P, Miklósi Á, Timár-Geng K, Csányi V (2004) Preference for copying unambiguous demonstrations in dogs (Canis familiaris). J Comp Psychol 118:375–383

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prato-Previde P, Marshall-Pescini S, Valsecci P (2008) Is your choice my choice? The owners’ effect on pet dogs’ (Canis lupus familiaris) performance in a food choice task. Anim Cogn 11:167–174

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Range F, Virányi Zs, Huber L (2007) Selective imitation in domestic dogs. Curr Biol 17:1–6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Range F, Heucke SL, Gruber C, Konz A, Huber L, Zs Virányi (2009a) The effect of ostensive cues on dogs’ performance in a manipulative social learning task. Appl Anim Behav Sci 120:170–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Range F, Horn L, Bugnyar T, Gajdon GK, Huber L (2009b) Social attention in keas, dogs, and human children. Anim Cogn 12:181–192

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reid PJ (2009) Adapting to the human world: dogs’ responsiveness to our social cues. Behav Processes 80:325–333

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ricciardelli P, Baylis G, Drivera J (2000) The positive and negative of human expertise in gaze perception. Cognition 77:B1–B14

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riedel J, Schumann K, Kaminski J, Call J, Tomasello M (2008) The early ontogeny of human-dog communication. Anim Behav 73:1003–1014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat MJ, Serra E, Fadiga L, Gallese V (2008) The evolution of social cognition: goal familiarity shapes monkeys’ action understanding. Curr Biol 18:227–232

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwier C, van Maanen C, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2006) Rational imitation in 12-month-old infants. Infancy 10:303–311

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senju A, Csibra G (2008) Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Curr Biol 18:668–671

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju A, Johnson MH, Csibra G (2006) The development and neural basis of referential gaze perception. Soc Neurosci 1:220–234

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szetei V, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2003) When dogs seem to loose their nose: an investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner. Appl Anim Behav Sci 83:141–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie C, Glabsch E, Tempelmann S, Brauer J, Kaminski J, Call J (2009) Dogs fail to copy intransitive actions in third party response facilitation tasks. Anim Behav 77:1491–1499

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teuscher C, Triesch J (2007) To each his own: the caregiver’s role in a computational model of gaze following. Neurocomputing 70:2166–2180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2008) Origins of human communication. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Haberl K (2003) Understanding attention: 12-and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Dev Psychol 39:906–912

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Kruger AC, Ratner HH (1993) Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 16:495–552

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Topál J, Csányi V (1994) The effect of eye-like schema on shuttling activity of wild house mice (Mus musculus domesticus)–context dependent threatening aspects of the eyespot patterns. Anim Learn Behav 22:96–102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Topál J, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á, Virányi Zs, Kubinyi E, Csányi V (2005) The effect of domestication and socialization on attachment to human: a comparative study on hand reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies. Anim Behav 70:1367–1375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Topál J, Byrne RW, Miklósi Á, Csányi V (2006) Reproducing human actions and action sequences: “Do as I Do!” in a dog. Anim Cogn 9:355–367

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Topál J, Gergely Gy, Miklósi Á, Erdőhegyi Á, Csibra G (2008) Infants perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science 321:1831–1834

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Topál J, Miklósi Á, Gácsi M, Dóka A, Pongrácz P, Kubinyi E, Virányi Zs, Csányi V (2009a) Dog as a complementary model for understanding human social behavior. Adv Study Behav 39:71–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Topál J, Gergely Gy, Erdőhegyi Á, Csibra G, Miklósi Á (2009b) Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves and human infants. Science 325:1269–1272

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Virányi Zs, Gácsi M, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Belényi B, Ujfalussy D, Miklósi Á (2008) Comprehension of the human pointing gesture in young socialized wolves and dogs. Anim Cogn 11:373–388

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warneken F, Tomasello M (2007) Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy 11:271–294

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood N, Glynn DD, Philips BC, Hauser M (2007) The perception of rational goal-directed action in nonhuman primates. Science 317(5843):1402–1405

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoon JMD, Johnson MH, Csibra G (2008) Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105(36):13690–13695

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank T. Vándor and K. Bayer for their assistance in data collection, I. Király, G. Csibra and Zs. Virányi for helpful comments and Ludwig Huber for giving the opportunity to work in the Clever Dog Lab, Vienna University. We also thank A. Kosztolányi for his help in the statistical analyses. Funded by the Hungarian Science Foundation (OTKA K76043) and by the EU6 Framework Programme grant (EDICI #12929).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Krisztina Kupán.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kupán, K., Miklósi, Á., Gergely, G. et al. Why do dogs (Canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task?. Anim Cogn 14, 259–268 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0359-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010-0359-0

Keywords

Navigation