Skip to main content
Log in

Exposure to an unpleasant odour increases the sense of Presence in virtual reality

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Virtual Reality Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

While olfactory cues affect the everyday human experience in the physical world, few studies have empirically examined the effect they could have on the human experience in virtual reality (VR). This project’s goal was to determine whether the exposure to olfactory stimuli would affect the senses of Presence (primary measure), Reality and Realism (exploratory measures) in VR. In a virtual kitchen devoid of obvious visual cues linking the visual scene to an odour, three groups of 20 randomly assigned participants (12 females and 8 males per group), unaware of the potential exposure to olfactory stimuli, were exposed to either ambient air, a pleasant odour, or an unpleasant odour. The results reveal that the unpleasant odour had a statistically significant effect on the sense of Presence (as measured by repeated brief measures of Presence and the Independent Television Commission Sense of Presence Inventory), but the pleasant one did not. The lower perceived intensity of the pleasant odour may have contributed to its lower detection rate which, in turn, may have contributed to the pleasant odour’s lack of effect on the sense of Presence. Neither of the olfactory stimuli had an effect on either the sense of Reality or the sense of Realism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alaoui-Ismaïli O, Vernet-Maury E, Dittmar A, Delhomme G, Chanel J (1997) Odor hedonics: connection with emotional response estimated by autonomic parameters. Chem Senses 22:237–248. doi:10.1093/chemse/22.3.237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aymerich-Franch L (2010) Presence and emotions in playing a group game in a virtual environment: the influence of body participation. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 13(6):649–654. doi:10.1089/cyber.2009.0412

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bangay S, Preston L (1998) An investigation into factors influencing immersion in interactive virtual reality environments. In: Riva G, Wiederhold BK, Molinari E (eds) Virtual environments in clinical psychology and neuroscience: methods and techniques in advanced patient-therapist interaction, vol 58. IOS Press, Amsterdam, pp 43–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Baños RM, Botella C, Garcia-Palacios A, Villa H, Perpiña C, Alcañiz M (2000) Presence and reality judgment in virtual environments: a unitary construct? Cyberpsychol Behav 3(3):327–335. doi:10.1089/10949310050078760

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barfield W, Danas E (1996) Comments on the use of olfactory displays for virtual environments. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 5(1):109–121

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baus O, Bouchard S (2010) The sense of olfaction: its characteristics and its possible applications in virtual environments. J CyberTherapy Rehabil 3(1):31–50

    Google Scholar 

  • Baus O, Bouchard S (2014) Moving from virtual reality exposure-based therapy to augmented reality exposure-based therapy: a review. Front Hum Neurosci 8:112. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard S, Robillard G, St-Jacques J, Dumoulin S, Patry MJ, Renaud P (2004) Reliability and validity of a single-item measure of presence in VR. IEEE international workshop on haptic virtual environments and their applications 3(October): 59–31

  • Bouchard S, St-Jacques J, Robillard G, Renaud P (2008) Anxiety increases the sense of presence in virtual reality. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 4(1):376–391. doi:10.1162/pres.17.4.376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cater JP (1992) The nose have it! Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 1(4):493–494

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen Y (2006) Olfactory display: development and application in virtual reality therapy. In: Proceedings of the 16th international conference on artificial reality and telexistence-Workshops (ICAT’06). http://www.computer.org/csdl/proceedings/icat/2006/2754/00/27540580-abs.html. Retrieved from 14 June 2012

  • Delplanque S, Grandjean D, Chrea C, Aymard L, Cayeux I, Le Calve B, Velazco MI, Scherer KR, Sander D (2008) Emotional processing of odors: evidence for a nonlinear relation between pleasantness and familiarity evaluations. Chem Senses 33(5):469–479. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjn014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demattè ML, Sanabria D, Spence C (2009) Olfactory identification: when vision matters? Chem Senses 34(2):103–109. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjn055

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinh HQ, Walker N, Song C, Kobayashi A, Hodges LF (1999) Evaluating the importance of multi-sensory input on memory and the sense of presence in virtual environments. Proc IEEE Virtual Real 1999:222–228. doi:10.1109/VR.1999.756955

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doop M, Mohr C, Folley B, Brewer WJ, Park S (2006) Olfaction and memory. In: Brewer WJ, Castle D, Pantelis C (eds) Olfaction and the brain. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrier L, Streff A, Martin S, Brouillet D, Barkat-Defradas M, Jiménez M (2009) Influence des stimuli olfactifs dans une tâche d’évaluation hédonique de couleurs : les yeux voient ce que le nez sent. L’Année Psychologique 109(3):361–381. doi:10.4074/S0003503309003017

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman J, Avons SE, Meddis R, Pearson D, IJsselsteijn W (2000) Using behavioral realism to estimate presence: a study of the utility of postural responses to motion stimuli. Presence 9(2):149–164. doi:10.1162/105474600566691

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heeter C (1992) Being there: the subjective experience of presence. Presence 1(2):262–271

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendrix C, Barfield W (1996) Presence within virtual environments as a function of visual display parameters. Presence 4(3):274–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsch AR (1995) Effects of ambient odors on slot-machine usage in a Las Vegas casino. Psychol Mark 12:585–594. doi:10.1002/mar.4220120703

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • IJsselsteijn W, Harper B (2001) Virtually there: a vision on presence research. EC Public deliverable, Presence Research Working Group. ftp://cordis.europa.eu/pub/ist/docs/fet/fetpr-4.pdf. Retrieved from 13 June 2012

  • Jackman AH, Doty RL (2009) Utility of a three-item smell identification test in detecting olfactory dysfunction. Laryngoscope 115(12):2209–2212. doi:10.1097/01.mlg.0000183194.17484.bb

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob TJC, Wang L (2006) A new method for measuring reaction times for odour detection at iso-intensity: comparison between an unpleasant and pleasant odour. Physiol Behav 87:500–505. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.11.018

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson GA (2005) The modified Tellegen absorption scale: a clearer window on the structure and meaning of absorption. Aust J Clin Exp Hypn 33(2):119–139

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson GA, Loi N (2013) An empirical test of Tellegen’s model of absorption: instrumental and experiential sets and the phenomenology of trance induction. http://une-au.academia.edu/GrahamJamieson/Papers. Retrieved from 10 Dec 2015

  • Jones L, Bowers CA, Washburn D, Cortes A, Satya RV (2004) The effect of olfaction on immersion into virtual environments. In: Vincenzi DA, Mouloua M, Hancock PA (eds) Human performance, situation awareness and automation: issues and considerations for the 21st century. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp 282–285. http://www.dtic.mil/cgi-bin/GetTRDoc?AD=ADA426023#page=292. Retrieved from 14 June 2012

  • Kalawsky RS (2000) The validity of presence as a reliable human performance metric in immersive environments. Paper presented at Presence 2000: International Workshop on Presence, Delft, Netherlands

  • Kennedy RS, Lane NE, Berbaum KS, Lilienthal MG (1993) Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. Int J Aviat Psychol 3(3):203–220. doi:10.1207/s15327108ijap0303_3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy RS, Stanney KM, Dunlap WP (2000) Duration and exposure to virtual environments: sickness curves during and across sessions. Presence 9(5):466–475. doi:10.1162/105474600566952

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirk-Smith MD, Booth DA (1990) The effect of five odorants on mood and the judgments of others. In: Macdonald DW, Müller-Schwarze D, Natynczuk S (eds) Chemical signals in vertebrates 5. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 48–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Köster EP, Degel J, Piper D (2002) Proactive and retroactive interference in implicit odor memory. Chem Senses 27:191–207. doi:10.1093/chemse/27.3.191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laurel B (1993) Computers as theatre. Addison-Wesley Longman Publishing Co., Inc., Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauria R (1997) Virtual reality: an empirical-metaphysical testbed. J Comput Mediat Commun 3(2). http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol3/issue2/lauria.html. Retrieved from 14 June 2012

  • Lee KM (2004) Presence, explicated. Commun Theory 14:27–50. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00302.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lessiter J, Freeman J, Keogh E, Davidoff J (2001) A cross-media presence questionnaire: the ITC-sense of presence inventory. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 10:282–297. doi:10.1162/105474601300343612

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li W, Moallem I, Paller KA, Gottfried JA (2007) Subliminal smells can guise social preferences. Psychol Sci 18(12):1044–1049. doi:10.111/j.1467-9280.2007.02023.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard M, Ditton TB (1997) At the heart of it all: the concept of presence. J Comput Mediat Commun 3(2). http://www.ascusc.org/jcmc/vol3/issue2/lombard.html. Retrieved from 13 June 2012

  • Mantovani F, Castelnuovo G (2003) The sense of presence in virtual training: enhancing skills acquisition and transfer of knowledge through learning experience in virtual environments. In: Davide F, Riva G, IJsselsteijn WA (eds) Being there: concepts, effects and measurement of user presence in synthetic environments. Ios Press, Amsterdam, pp 167–182

    Google Scholar 

  • Minsky M (1980) Telepresence. Omni 2(9):45–51. doi:10.1145/566654.566630

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicovich SG, Boller GW, Cornwell TB (2005) Experienced presence within computer medicated communications: initial explorations on the effects of gender with respect to empathy and immersion. J Comput Mediat Commun 10(6). http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol10/issue2/nicovich.html. Retrieved from 14 June 2012

  • Pratt DR, Zyda M, Kelleher K (1995) Virtual reality: in the mind of the beholder. IEEE Comput 28(7):17–19. doi:10.1109/MC.1995.10085

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratey JJ (2001) A user’s guide to the brain. Pantheon Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Rattaz C, Goubet N, Bullinger A (2001) The calming effect of a familiar odor following a painful experience. Poster presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN

  • Sacau A, Laarni J, Hartmann T (2008) Influence of individual factors on presence. Comput Hum Behav 24:2255–2273. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2007.11.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez-Vives MV, Slater M (2005) From presence to consciousness through virtual reality. Nat Rev Neurosci 6(4):332–339. doi:10.1038/nrn1651

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sas C (2004) Individual differences in virtual environments. In Bubak M, van Albada GD, Sloot PMA, Dongarra J (eds) Lecture notes in computer science: vol. 3038. Computational science—ICCS 2004. Berlin, Springer, Germany, pp 1017–1024. doi:10.1007/b97989

  • Schank RC (1997) Virtual learning: a revolutionary approach to building a highly skilled workforce. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffman SS (1974) Psychochemical correlates of olfactory quality. Science 185(146):112–117. doi:10.1126/science.185.4146.112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert T, Friedmann F, Regenbrecht H (2001) The experience of presence: factor analytic insights. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 10(3):266–281. doi:10.1162/105474601300343603

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuemie MJ (2003) Human–computer interaction and presence in virtual reality exposure therapy. Doctoral dissertation, Technische Universiteit Delft, Holland. http://graphics.tudelft.nl/~vrphobia/dissertation.pdf. Retrieved from 13 June 2012

  • Seigneuric A, Durand K, Jiang T, Baudouin J-Y, Schaal B (2010) The nose tells it to the eyes: crossmodal associations between olfaction and vision. Perception 39(11):1541–1554. doi:10.1068/p6740

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheridan TB (1992) Musings on telepresence and virtual presence. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 1(1):120–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel S (1999) Multiple chemical sensitivity as a conditional response. Toxicol Ind Health 15:323–330. doi:10.1177/074823379901500306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slater M (2003) A note on presence terminology, Presence connect, 3. http://people.cs.uct.ac.za/~dnunez/reading/papers/presenceterminology.pdf. Retrieved from 13 June 2012

  • Slater M (2004) How colorful was your day? Why questionnaires cannot assess presence in virtual environments. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 13(4):484–493. doi:10.1162/1054746041944849

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slater M, Usoh M (1993) Representations systems, perceptual position, and presence in immersive virtual environments. Presence Teleoper Virtual Environ 2(3):221–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slater M, Wilbur S (1995) Through the looking glass world of presence: a framework for immersive virtual environments. In: Slater M (ed) FIVE’95 framework for immersive virtual environments. QMW University, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater M, Lotto B, Arnold MM, Sanchez-Vives MV (2009) How we experience immersive virtual environments: the concept of presence and its measurement. Anuario de Psicología 40(2):193–210

    Google Scholar 

  • Steuer J (1995) Defining virtual reality: dimensions determining telepresence. In: Biocca F, Levy MR (eds) Communication in the age of virtual reality. L.E.A, Englewood, pp 33–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen A, Atkinson G (1974) Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (‘absorption’), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J Abnorm Psychol 83(3):268–277. doi:10.1037/h0036681

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornson CA, Goldiez BF, Le H (2009) Predicting presence: constructing the tendency toward presence inventory. Int J Hum Comput Stud 67:62–78. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2008.08.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Schaik P, Turnbull T, van Wersch A, Drummond S (2004) Presence within a mixed reality environment. CyberPsychol Behav 7:540–552. doi:10.1089/cpb.2004.7.540

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Washburn DA, Jones LM, Vijaya Satya R, Bowers CA, Cortes A (2003) Olfactory use in virtual environment training. Model Simul Mag 2(3):19–25

    Google Scholar 

  • Witmer BG, Singer MJ (1998) Measuring presence in virtual environments: a presence questionnaire. Presence 7(3):225–240. doi:10.1162/105474698565686

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zybura M, Eskeland GA (1999) Olfaction for virtual reality. Quarter Project, Industrial Engineering 543. University of Washington, Winter 1999. http://www.hitl.washington.edu/people/tfurness/courses/inde543/reports/3doc. Retrieved from 14 June 2012

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) scholarship awarded to the first author, as well as by grants from the NSERC, the Canada Foundation for Innovation (CFI) and the Canada Research Chairs awarded to the second author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Oliver Baus.

Appendix: Additional analyses

Appendix: Additional analyses

1.1 Characteristics of olfactory stimuli detected in virtuo

1.1.1 AMB group

For this group, although no olfactory stimulus was generated, the in virtuo detection rate of an olfactory stimulus in the virtual kitchen was 5 % (1 of 20). More specifically, this male participant reported that the odour was associated with a memory and rated the odour as follows: Pleasantness = 2.00, Intensity = 4.00, Familiarity = 3.00, and Concordance to the kitchen = 1.00.

1.1.2 PLE and UNP groups

Detection rates, rate of association with a memory, and presumed source of odours are shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Detection, association with memory, and source of odours detected in virtuo

1.1.3 Comparison between the characteristics of PLE and UNP odours

Due to the inequality of the size of the groups, and in order to compare the characteristics of the PLE and UNP odours, nonparametric comparisons were conducted. These confirmed that, as assessed by the PLE (n = 3) and UNP (n = 12) participants who detected the odour in virtuo, the UNP odours were statistically significantly more unpleasant (Mdn = 4.00) than the PLE odour (Mdn = 1.00; U = 0.00, z = −2.68, p = .01, d = 4.64, r = −.92). However, the differences between the PLE and UNP odours were not statistically different either in terms of Intensity (respectively, Mdns = 5.00 and 5.50; U = 7.50, z = −1.66, p = .10, d = 1.19, r = .51), Familiarity (respectively, Mdns = 4.00 and 2.50; U = 6.50, z = −1.71, p = .09, d = −1.11, r = −.49), or Concordance with the visual scene (respectively, Mdns = 3.00 and 2.00; U = 12.50, z = −.82, p = .41, d = −.58, r = −.58). Mean values and standard errors are shown in Fig. 8.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Characteristics of olfactory stimuli detected in virtuo. These are based on the participants who reported to have detected an odour in the kitchen (for the pleasant odour, n = 3; for unpleasant odour, n = 12). Standard errors are represented in the figure by the error bars attached to each column

1.2 Characteristics of olfactory stimuli detected in vivo

1.2.1 PLE group

The post-experiment in vivo detection rate of the same olfactory stimulus these participants had been exposed to in the kitchen was 100 % (20 of 20). Fifteen of the group members (75 %) stated that the odour was associated with a memory.

1.2.2 UNP group

The post-experiment in vivo detection rate of the same olfactory stimulus these participants had been exposed to in the kitchen was 100 % (20 of 20). Eighteen of the group members (90 %) stated that the odour was associated with a memory.

1.2.3 Comparison between the characteristics of PLE and UNP odours

Due to the non-normal distribution of the characteristics of the odours, and in order to compare the characteristics of the PLE and UNP odours, nonparametric comparisons were conducted. These confirmed that, as assessed by the participants of the PLE (n = 20) and UNP (n = 20) groups when they were exposed to the odour in vivo (post-questionnaires), the UNP odour was statistically significantly less pleasant (Mdn = 0.00) than the PLE odour (Mdn = 4.00; U = 0.00, z = −5.54, p = .00, d = 4.42, r = .91), more intense than the PLE odour (respectively, Mdns = 6.00 and 5.00; U = 122.50, z = −2.20, p = .03, d = −.82, r = −.38), less familiar than the PLE odour (respectively, Mdns = 3.50 and 5.00; U = 113.00, z = −2.43, p = .02, d = .93, r = .42), and less concordant with the visual scene than the PLE odour (respectively, Mdns = 1.00 and 3.00; U = 121.00, z = −2.19, p = .03, d = .74, r = .35). Mean values and standard errors are shown in Fig. 9.

Fig. 9
figure 9

Characteristics of olfactory stimuli detected in vivo. These are based on a 30-s exposure to the same stimulus the participants had been exposed to in virtuo. This time, however, participants had been made aware of the exposure to an olfactory stimulus (n = 20 for each of the groups). Standard errors are represented in the figure by the error bars attached to each column

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Baus, O., Bouchard, S. Exposure to an unpleasant odour increases the sense of Presence in virtual reality. Virtual Reality 21, 59–74 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-016-0299-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-016-0299-3

Keywords

Navigation