Introduction

While physiological changes in breathing normally occur during the different sleep phases, more profound breathing alterations are observed in some pathological sleep disorders, such as in obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) (Atanasov and Dimov 2003). OSA is characterized by repeated episodes of closure or stenosis of the upper respiratory tract, leading to apnea or hypopnea with either preserved normal or increased respiratory muscle activity. In adults, the clinical history of OSA includes nocturnal symptoms such as periods of apnea, interrupted by periods of loud snoring, restless sleep, and sudden awakenings. Additionally, daytime symptoms such as increased sleepiness, morning headaches, and impaired attention have been observed (Guilleminault et al. 1977). Obesity is the most common cause of OSA in adults (Chen et al. 2014), while other factors, such as high neck circumference, craniofacial and upper airway anatomical abnormalities, can also increase the likelihood of developing OSA (Borges et al. 2014). OSA requires differentiation with Obesity Hypoventilation Syndrome (OHS) which concerns obese patients, presenting clinical signs of OSA but not confirmed in polysomnography (Zawada et al. 2013; Ryan et al. 2014; Liu and Yuan 2014).

The clinical picture of OSA in children or adolescents is different to adults. Children with OSA present with loud snoring, together with other, non-specific symptoms, including: growth retardation, enuresis, academic learning difficulties, and behavioral problems, such as attention deficit/hyperactivity (ADHD) disorders. Unlike obesity in adults, tonsillar and adenoid hypertrophy is the most common etiology of OSA in children, followed by anatomical abnormalities (Solyom et al. 2014). Despite the different clinical presentations, polysomnography remains the gold standard for diagnosing OSA in adults and children, in addition to clinical history (Health Quality Ontario 2006).

As a consequence of OSA, decreased blood oxygen saturation, and conscious or unconscious arousal (wake-up events) can occur during the night (Paiva and Altarian 2014). If left untreated, OSA may cause significant neurological problems, including stroke, depression, headaches, peripheral neuropathy, and non-arteritic ischemic optic neuropathy (NAION). Moreover, OSA is often associated with impaired cognitive function, most likely due to the intermittent hypoxia.

A relationship between OSA and cognitive impairment was first identified in the 1980s (Findley et al. 1986; Greenberg et al. 1987; Klonoff et al. 1987). However, despite many years of research in this field, the prevalence, pathogenesis, and therapy of impaired cognitive functions relating to OSA remains elusive. Moreover, in comparison to a wide range of publications devoted to cognition in the adult population with OSA, research on the neuropsychological effects of the pediatric form of the disorder is limited (Beebe et al. 2004; Horiuchi et al. 2014).

In this review, we aimed to determine the most affected cognitive domains in adults and children/adolescents with OSA, as evaluated by neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies. To do this, we identified previously published studies on sleep, breathing disorders, and cognitive dysfunction from 1990 until 2015 the PubMed database using the following search terms [effective date: August 26, 2015): (sleep (Title/Abstract) AND apnea (Title/Abstract)] OR breathing disorders (Title/Abstract) OR cognitive deficits (Title/Abstract) OR attention (Title/Abstract) OR executive functions AND cognitive deficits (Title/Abstract)] OR children (Title/Abstract) OR adolescents OR adults OR CPAP OR polysomnography AND English (lang) AND (1990/01/01(PDAT): 2015/08/26PDAT)). We identified 26 original studies referring to adults, and 15 original studies referring to children and adolescents. We also included systematic reviews, meta-analyses, case reports, etc., in our discussion.

Cognitive domains affected in adults with OSA

Overviews of the identified studies investigating cognitive deficits due to OSA in the adult population are presented in Tables 1 and 2. Unfortunately, the exact prevalence of the cognitive dysfunctions in adults with OSA is still unknown and varies widely depending on study design. For example, in one study (Saunamäki et al. 2009), most OSA patients performed at a normal level; only few patients had mild (2.5–12.5 %) or moderate-to-severe (5–15 %) cognitive dysfunction. In contrast, a prospective observational study reported that one in four newly diagnosed OSA patients had a severe and distinctive neuropsychological dysfunction (Antonelli Incalzi et al. 2004). Finally, Pierobon et al. (2008) reported that at least 59.2 % of obese OSA patients had at least one cognitive impairment. Therefore a large, multi-center study on the cognitive deficits in OSA patients, with and without obesity, is still required to accurately determine its prevalence.

Table 1 Neuropsychological assessment of cognitive impairments in adults with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome—an overview of selected studies published after year 2000
Table 2 Neuroimaging assessment of adults with obstructive sleep apnea—an overview of selected studies published after year 2000

According to a recent meta-analysis (Bucks et al. 2013), most published studies have identified executive functions, attention, and memory as the most affected cognitive domains in OSA. Moreover, a systematic review published by Saunamäki and Jehkonen (2007) stated that executive functioning is the most impaired cognitive domain in OSA in the adult population (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Impact of OSA on cognitive impairment

Executive function is impaired in adults with OSA

Executive function is the control of cognitive processes, including working memory, reasoning, task flexibility, problem solving, planning, and execution. Deficits in executive function in adults with OSA are mostly related to working memory, but also to phonological fluency, cognitive flexibility, and planning (Saunamäki et al. 2009). However, while the executive functions in the systematic review by Saunamäki and Jehkonen (2007) were generally assessed with standardized test methods, the sample sizes were significantly varied and the groups were widely distributed in terms of OSA severity. Nemeth et al. (2012) suggested that there is selective susceptibility of OSA patients to the more attention-demanding cognitive functions, such as working memory and executive functions, compared to general skill learning and sequence-specific learning. Moreover, when Lis et al. (2008) separated the deficits in executive functions into the more elementary cognitive processes involved in task solving, they found differences in processing time derived from the deficits at a more basic cognitive processing level. However, this reduced accuracy appeared to be attributed specifically to working memory.

On the other hand, one study (Borges et al. 2013) on a selected group of OSA patients without comorbidities reported no significant group differences in test scores measuring six distinct executive function domains (shifting, inhibition, updating, dual-task performance, planning, and access to long-term memory). The authors of this study concluded that OSA without comorbidities did not lead to executive function impairment. However, Hilsendager et al. (2015) reported that OSA combined with obesity does lead to executive function impairment, and that obesity status is a significant predictor of performance in the executive functioning tasks.

In summary, the previously published studies suggest that working memory is the most commonly affected subpart of executive functioning in adults. However, it remains to be established whether these changes are related to OSA symptoms or the comorbidities, such as obesity, which lead to cardiovascular and neurological complications. A valuable assessment of working memory using one consistent neuropsychological test, supported by neuroimaging studies discussed below, should be performed in order to confirm these findings.

Attention deficits are common in adults with OSA

Several studies have demonstrated that OSA patients show impairments in sustained, selective, and divided attention (Bucks et al. 2013; Gagnon et al. 2014). It has been suggested that these vigilance and attention deficits could influence and worsen other aspects of cognitive deficits in OSA, such as executive functioning and episodic memory impairments (Gagnon et al. 2014). However, most of these studies included only moderate and severe OSA patients, while mild OSA subjects were generally excluded.

Not all aspects of attention are equally impaired in OSA patients. In one study (Tulek et al. 2013), attentional control processes were found to be impaired when focal attention (Flanker task) processes were involved, but were intact when observed using the Simon and Stroop tasks. Moreover, Sforza et al. (2004) found that the speed with which the subjects respond to a visual stimulus (measured with the psychomotor vigilance task) was more affected than the accuracy in OSA patients.

As Mazza et al. (2005) hypothesized that a single test could underestimate impaired vigilance and attention in some patients, they performed three attentional tests (measuring maintenance of wakefulness, as well as sustained, selective, and divided attention) at three time points (9 am, 11 am, and 1:30 pm) within the same day. OSA patients performed significantly worse in all three tests during the three sessions. Additionally, vigilance and/or attentional impairments were found in 95 % of OSA patients. While the results of this study show a high percentage of OSA patients with attentional deficits, the results could have been influenced by morning somnolence, which is more pronounced in OSA patients. Therefore, it would be valuable to perform a battery of attentional deficits tests, at different times of the day and while performing different activities, in both mild and moderate/severe cases of OSA.

Memory and learning deficits are often mild in adults with OSA

Most studies report that memory impairment in OSA patients is mild (Yaouhi et al. 2009; Gelir et al. 2014), and not all memory processes are affected (Beebe et al. 2003; Naëgelé et al. 2006; Wallace and Bucks 2013). According to a recent meta-analysis (Wallace and Bucks 2013), subjects with OSA are more significantly affected in verbal episodic memory and visuo-spatial episodic memory, but not as much in visual immediate recall or visuo-spatial learning when compared to healthy controls. However, the authors emphasize the weaknesses in the methodology of the reviewed publications, such as significant heterogeneity in the sample size, oxygen levels, premorbid IQ or education, choice of comparison group, and a lack of classification for memory tasks. They also discuss the possible necessity to use screening polysomnography in the control group.

Twigg et al. (2010) reported that OSA is associated with an impairment in verbal, but not visual memory. This contrasts with the meta-analysis conducted by Beebe et al. (2003), who found no significant effect of OSA on verbal immediate recall and verbal long-term memory. Moreover, Naëgelé et al. (2006) investigated three separate memory systems (verbal episodic, procedural, and working) and found OSA patients exhibited a deficit of episodic memory, decreased performance in procedural memory, and an impairment of specific working memory capabilities. However, no correlation was found between OSA severity and memory deficits, which is in line with the results of Twigg et al. (2010). On the other hand, one group recently reported negative correlations between OSA severity using the apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) and immediate memory, logical memory, and theme drawings (Jurádo-Gámez et al. 2015). In contrast, Gelir et al. (2014) found no significant effect of OSA on working or procedural memory, which is in line with the study of Rouleau et al. (2002).

In summary, the data have been inconsistent in terms of impaired memory functioning in OSA patients. Such inconsistencies could be related to a difficulty in finding a proper diagnostic neuropsychological test that would precisely examine this type of dysfunction. It would be valuable to assess the cortical and cerebellar areas responsible for each type of memory system using a neuroimaging studies as a supplement to a valid neuropsychological test as discussed below.

Cognitive deficits in adults with OSA assessed by neuroimaging studies

Neuroimaging assessment in conjunction with neuropsychological studies can help us to confirm the cognitive deficits in OSA. The studies that have used such neuroimaging techniques are summarized in Table 2.

Zhang et al. (2011) assessed severe OSA patients with both neuropsychological and neuroimaging tests (i.e., functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI, during a brief visual delayed matching-to-sample task), and showed a reduced mismatch-related activation in frontal regions. This impairment correlated with the arousal index and hypoxia in these patients, suggesting that hypoxia may cause this cognitive deficit. In a similar study (Thomas et al. 2005), fMRI was used to map cerebral activation during a 2-back verbal working memory task. Working memory speed in OSA patients was significantly slower than in the control group and was associated with the absence of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation. Therefore, by combining these neuropsychological and neuroimaging tests, it was confirmed that working memory is affected in adults with OSA.

As the hippocampus and frontal cortex are closely associated with memory processes and executive functions, and are particularly susceptible to sleep fragmentation, hypoxemia and hypercapnia (Beebe and Gozal 2002), most studies show altered brain metabolism and neuronal loss in these areas in OSA patients. For example, Alchanatis et al. (2004) examined OSA patients using proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) and found that the N-acetylaspartate-to-creatine ratio (indicative of axonal loss and/or dysfunction) and the choline-to-creatine ratio (indicative of myelin lipid loss and or/phospholipid metabolism dysfunction) were significantly lower in the frontal white matter of OSA patients compared controls. In regards to the frontal lobe, white matter lesions are known to be associated with a cognitive executive dysfunction, which may explain the irreversibility of some cognitive deficits related to OSA. Moreover, a study performed on hippocampal area metabolites (Bartlett et al. 2004) reported that in the left hippocampal area, the N-acetyl-containing:creatine-containing compound ratio was significantly increased in OSA subjects. As there was no significant difference in the hippocampal N-acetyl-containing compounds levels, the observed alterations were most likely due to a decrease in creatine-containing compounds. These lower levels of hippocampal creatine-containing compounds were correlated with a worse OSA severity and neurocognitive performance. Finally, using voxel-based morphometry, Morrell et al. (2003) reported a significantly lower gray matter concentration within the left hippocampus in right-handed OSA patients compared to controls. The results of these studies indicate the brain morphology changes in the hippocampus are associated with OSA.

Similarly, Yaouhi et al. (2009) reported that significant cerebral changes, as assessed by MRI, precede the onset of neuropsychological impairment in patients with OSA. Indeed, indications of axonal injury in axons linking major structures within the limbic system, pons, frontal, temporal, and parietal cortices and projections to and from the cerebellum have been reported in OSA patients (Macey et al. 2008). Torelli et al. (2011) also evaluated brain morphological changes and their relationship to neuropsychological test results in moderate to severe OSA patients. The volumes of cortical gray matter, as well as the size of the right hippocampus, right and left caudate, were smaller in OSA patients compared to the control group and correlated with poorer performance in several neuropsychological tests. In addition, when brain activation during a response inhibition test (i.e., a Go/No-go task) was examined using fMRI, OSA patients showed lower ability to withhold a response, and this was associated with decreased brain activation in the left postcentral gyrus, cingulate gyrus, and inferior parietal lobe, right insula and putamen (Ayalon et al. 2009). Therefore, different areas in the brain can be morphologically affected in OSA patients, and these changes correspond to differing cognitive deficits.

By combining neuropsychological assessment and neuroimaging studies, we can precisely identify the areas in the brain responsible for cognitive impairments in OSA patients. There should be more focus on identifying the particular areas in the brain that are responsible during assessment of OSA patients with cognitive deficits. Such combined studies would help us to determine whether these changes are due to hypoxemic episodes of OSA or other causes.

Cognitive deficits in children and adolescents with OSA

While there are only limited studies on cognitive deficits in children and adolescents, the accessible findings show that the type and intensity of symptoms presented in young patients vary according to age. Unfortunately, while there is substantial evidence confirming the presence of a cognitive deficit in school-aged children, the number of studies examining pre-school children is much lower (Jackman et al. 2012). A summary of the available studies on the cognitive deficits in children and adolescents with OSA is shown in Table 3.

Table 3 Neuropsychological and neuroimaging assessment of cognitive impairments in children and adolescents with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome—an overview of selected studies published after year 2000

In one of their studies, Jackman et al. (2012) focused on children (aged 3–5 years) with primary snoring and mild/moderate/severe OSA syndrome compared to non-snoring children. Overnight polysomnography and cognitive battery were applied, along with behavioral analyses. The authors reported that sleep-disordered breathing (SDB) of any severity was associated with poorer behavior, but not decreased cognitive performance. The authors also drew attention to the possible study limitations, such as the fact that during cognitive assessments of young children, it is often more difficult to keep them engaged in assessment procedures as they are also more easily distracted than older children. Also, children of this age may often refuse to complete subtests or are not sufficiently cooperative.

Jackman et al. (2012) also suggested that early treatment of OSA may prevent cognitive deficits arising later in childhood. Unfortunately, there is evidence that cognitive impairment is already found in children as young as 5 years. For example, Gottlieb et al. (2004) found that SDB symptoms are associated with poorer executive function and memory skills and lower general intelligence in children aged 5 years (using a parent-completed questionnaire, polysomnography, and standardized neurocognitive tests). Moreover, when school-aged children with SDB were treated with adenotonsillectomy, improvements in SDB after 4 years were concomitant with improvements in some areas of neurocognition, but not in academic ability or behavior (Biggs et al., 2014). Therefore, an earlier diagnosis (before age 5) and treatment may have prevented some of these cognitive deficits from occurring.

As mentioned above, there have been more studies on school-aged children. Bourke et al. (2011) found that neurocognitive deficits are common in children with SDB (aged 7–12 years) regardless of the disease severity. Archbold et al. (2004) examined 12 children with mild SBD (aged 8.0–11.9 years) and found they showed significant impairment in sustained attention and vigilance on a computerized continuous performance test. While a number of studies present cognitive domains that are similarly affected in both adults and children, such as executive functioning or attention, specific impaired domains for the pediatric population, such as phonological processing, have also been reported. For example, O’Brien et al. (2004) showed that children with SDB scored significantly lower than controls on the attention/executive function domain, and specifically on the visual attention and executive function. In addition, children with SDB scored significantly lower than controls on phonological processing, the skill that is critical for learning how to read. In a study performed a few years later by Hamasaki Uema et al. (2007), it was observed that children aged 6–12 years with OSA show worse Rey test scores, which could indicate deficits in a number of cognitive domains, such as visuo-spatial memory, attention, and working memory. In addition, Lau et al. (2015) found that verbal working memory impairment related to OSA in children may have a further negative impact on learning potential and neurocognitive development. However, it is important to note that Biggs et al. (2011) examined 127 children aged 7–12 years with SDB to find out whether the deficits in memory were due to parental overestimation, and found that parents of children with less severe SDB tend to overestimate their symptoms. This suggests that observation of deficits in working memory may be largely dependent on the assessment method, and that children with SDB may not be as impaired as previously thought.

Some unique cognitive deficits in school-aged children with OSA have been described compared with the adult population. For example, Kheirandish-Gozal et al. (2014) showed that cognitive and empathetic processing (as measured by a fMRI during a color-word Stroop task and an empathy task) was deteriorated in 10 children (aged 7–11 years) with polysomnographically diagnosed OSA. However, a much larger sample size is required to confirm this finding.

Another study in school-aged children found that children with OSA have a higher arousal index than controls (Miano et al. 2011). Therefore, they hypothesized that arousal is a defensive mechanism that may preserve cognitive function by counteracting the respiratory events at the expense of sleep maintenance. Moreover, they reported that children with OSA had a higher ADHD rating, which negatively correlated with the level of oxygen saturation during the night. This suggests a correlation between attention deficits and OSA in children, most likely due to hypoxia.

Similar to the findings in adults, neuroimaging or neuroelectrophysiological studies can confirm the cognitive deficits in children with OSA. For example, Quan et al. (2013) examined children with SDB by neurocognitive battery and electroencephalographic monitoring, and showed that long-term changes in executive function were undetectable by neuropscyhological testing, but were visible during neuroelectrophysiological monitoring. Moreover, in a study by Barnes et al. (2012), children with OSA underwent overnight sleep studies and neuropscyhological testing, as well as the oddball attention task, while event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded. They concluded that specific ERPs during a single attention task can reliably identify the presence of OSA-associated cognitive dysfunction. According to authors, electrophysiological approaches during specific cognitive tasks may serve as simple, complementary, and reliable reporters of cognitive dysfunction associated with OSA in children.

The effects of obesity on cognitive deficits in children with OSA have also been investigated. In a study by Tan et al. (2014), 31 adolescents (aged 10–18 years) with OSA and obesity (mean BMI of 32.3 ± 4.9) were assessed according to intelligence, memory, learning, academic achievement, behavior, and executive functioning. The authors concluded that children with OSA and obesity may have an increased risk for some poorer educational and behavioral outcomes. In addition, Hannon et al. (2012) postulated that sleep fragmentation and poorer sleep quality have implications for neurocognitive functioning in obese adolescents. Similar conclusions were drawn by the research group from the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, who found that obese adolescents with OSA presented impaired executive and behavioral functions compared to controls, and were more likely to score in the clinically abnormal range on measures of neurobehavioral functioning (Xanthopoulos et al. 2014). Similarly, in the study performed by Vitelli et al. (2015), it was observed that in the group of children with OSA, the worst cognitive performance was found in the subgroup with comorbid obesity. According to the authors, these findings were very important, as this period of life is crucial for the development of proper frontal lobe functioning.

There are also other comorbidities described in the literature associated with OSA in children, such as Down syndrome (Lal et al. 2015). Among children with this condition, mean verbal IQ score was found to be lower in patients with comorbid OSA than in those without OSA, and performance on measures of cognitive flexibility was poorer (Breslin et al. 2014).

In summary, the cognitive dysfunctions in children focus on slightly different areas of cognition than in adults, such as behavioral and educational aspects, visual attention, and phonological processing, which are necessary for the proper intellectual development of a child. Based on the early changes in neuroimaging and neuropsychological presentation, these children should be treated early to prevent persistent, irreversible cognitive changes in the future. There should be more direct focus on assessing the cognitive functions in the youngest patients with OSA, however, due to their inability to verbally express their thoughts, there is limited ability for neuropsychological assessment.

OSA treatment methods

OSA treatment in adults using continuous positive airway pressure

In terms of treatment in the adult population, the dominant method remains continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP), which requires strong patient compliance (Hussain et al. 2014). However, CPAP studies in OSA patients show a significant discrepancy in results, most likely related to a large variability in study designs and durations of treatment. Several authors (Aloia et al. 2004; Saunamäki and Jekhonen 2007; Lau et al. 2010; Antic et al. 2011) report that even the most optimally treated OSA patients may not experience a complete reversal in attention and executive functions, due to the permanent brain alterations in severe cases (Ferini-Strambi et al. 2003). It confirms the findings of the apnea positive pressure long-term efficacy study (APPLES), a recent, randomized, double-blind multi-center trial (Kushida et al. 2012) on 1098 participants diagnosed with OSA. APPLES reported that neurocognitive measures (attention and psychomotor function, verbal learning and memory, executive and frontal-lobe function) did not improve after 6 months of CPAP therapy compared to sham CPAP therapy. However, results of an earlier study (Felver-Gant et al. 2007) indicated that patients who were highly compliant with treatment demonstrated selective improvement in working memory after 3 months of CPAP treatment. Additionally, in a study on severe OSA patients (Ferini-Strambi et al. 2003) authors reported that after 15-days of CPAP treatment, attentive, visuo-spatial learning, and motor performances returned to normal baseline levels, but after a further 4 months of CPAP treatment did not result in any further improvement in cognitive tests. Moreover, they found that performance during tests evaluating executive functions and constructional abilities was not affected by short- and long-term treatment with CPAP.

One study showed that attention and vigilance, psychomotor speed, executive functioning, and non-verbal delayed recall improved in patients compliant with CPAP treatment after 3 months (Aloia et al. 2003). However, the results from studies investigating the effects of CPAP on memory are inconsistent. Grenèche et al. (2013) investigated the effects of CPAP treatment on short-term memory performance over 24 h of sustained wakefulness and revealed that untreated OSA patients had no deficit in the immediate memory but were impaired in working memory, which was improved after 6 months of CPAP treatment.

As the abnormalities in cognitive functions are reflected in structural brain changes, O’Donoghue et al. (2005) investigated the effect of CPAP treatment on brain structure. The authors reported no longitudinal changes in gray matter density or regional volumes after treatment, but whole brain volume decreased slightly. In contrast, Canessa et al. (2011) observed significant improvements involving memory, attention, and executive functioning that paralleled with gray-matter volume increases in hippocampal and frontal structures after 3 months of CPAP treatment. White matter integrity measured by diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and cognitive performance was also found to improve in compliant patients after 12 months of CPAP treatment (Castronovo et al. 2014).

The promising results and return of some cognitive functions after compliant CPAP therapy indicate that more research in this area is warranted. Together with neuroimaging evaluation and high compliance to treatment, it would be possible to establish the proper duration, intensity and frequency of CPAP treatment in patients with cognitive dysfunctions.

OSA treatment in children using adenotonsillectomy and positive airway pressure

In the pediatric population, adenotonsillectomy is considered the most effective treatment (Lee et al. 2014). The most important clinical study on this treatment method, the CHAT clinical trial, was published in 2013 (Marcus et al. 2013). The CHAT study evaluated the usefulness of early adenotonsillectomy in a children aged 5–9 years in terms of a reduction in symptoms compared with a control group managed by watchful waiting and supportive care treatment. After evaluating 464 children, the authors concluded that surgical treatment for the OSA in school-age children did not significantly improve attention or executive function, as measured by neuropsychological testing, compared to watchful waiting management. However, adenotonsillectomy did reduce symptoms and improve secondary outcomes of behavior, quality of life, and polysomnographic findings. Similarly, Esteller et al. (2014) performed a study to analyze the outcomes of cognitive and behavioral disorders 1-year post-adenotonsillectomy. They found that behavioral and cognitive disturbances in children with OSA (aged 3–13 years) were partially resolved following adenotonsillectomy. However, improvements in the cognitive and behavioral variables did not significantly differ from the normal development of an individual, and were independent of the resolution of respiratory disorders.

Yuan et al. (2012) proposed that neurocognitive functioning in children with OSA may be improved with the use of positive airway pressure (PAP). Participants in their study were examined with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV, the Delis Kaplan Executive Functioning Scales, the Test of Everyday Attention for Children, and the Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning-2nd Edition to assess the neurocognitive function. Participants prior to therapy reflected neurocognitive deficiencies in all areas. Of the original 21 children, four completed the full PAP treatment and were re-evaluated, demonstrating improvements in memory and motor speed. While this study shows promising results, the sample size is too small to make any assumptions. Therefore, more research in the field of PAP treatment in children with neurocognitive dysfunctions due to OSA is required.

Conclusions

Cognitive deficits related to breathing disorders have been observed in adults, children and adolescents, and show a wide range of neuropsychological presentations. In children, the symptoms may present as problems with working memory, attention, or executive functions cognitive domains, but also as changes in behavior and phonological processing necessary for proper development. However, if treated properly and early, these changes may be reversible (Idiazabal-Alecha and Fernandez-Prats 2014). Altered cognition accompanying breathing disorders has also been observed in adults (Varga et al. 2014) and elderly patients (Martin et al. 2014), and executive functions, attention and memory are the most affected cognitive domains in OSA (Bucks et al. 2013). These symptoms may be additionally complicated by depression and anxiety, which are more increasingly present in the adult population (Rezaeitalab et al. 2014). Despite much research, the prevalence and pathogenesis of breathing disorders in the different age groups remains unclear. Also, there is a need to set standardized protocols for the treatment of impaired cognitive functions associated with OSA. As highlighted in this review, the last decades of research in this field have resulted in inconsistent data, in part because of small sample sizes, different study designs, as well as varying test batteries, exclusion and inclusion criteria, and treatment durations. The literature devoted to cognition in OSA in children and adolescents is very limited and more studies should be performed on specific pediatric age groups. Finally, while PAP and adenotonsillectomy are currently the most effective treatments for OSA, further study is required to establish the most optimal treatment approach for each age group to prevent cognitive dysfunctions.