Skip to main content
Log in

Characteristics of tummy time and dose-response relationships with development in infants

  • Research
  • Published:
European Journal of Pediatrics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study aimed to examine change in tummy time patterns and preferences in the first 6 months of life, as well as dose–response relationships between tummy time duration and development at 2, 4, and 6 months. Participants were parents of infants from the Early Movers project in Edmonton, Canada (baseline: n = 411). At 2, 4, and 6 months, infant tummy time duration and preference (i.e., 1 = really likes to 5 = really dislikes) and development (i.e., Ages & Stages Questionnaire (ASQ-3) communication, fine motor, gross motor, problem-solving, personal-social) were measured by a parental questionnaire. In a subsample (n = 127), tummy time patterns (i.e., bout frequency, mean and median bout length) were measured using a 3-day time-use diary. Tummy time bout frequency, bout length, and preference significantly increased over time. Linear dose–response relationships between tummy time duration and development outcomes were observed at 4 (gross motor) and 6 months (all development outcomes). Moreover, at 2 months, 30–44 min/day of tummy time was associated with a higher total development score (vs. < 15 min/day; B = 11.14; 95%CI: 1.60, 20.68). At 6 months, 61–120 min/day (vs. < 30 min/day; B = 27.12; 95%CI: 11.93, 42.32) and > 120 min/day (vs. < 30 min/day; B = 33.80; 95%CI: 18.90, 48.70) of tummy time were associated with higher total development scores. Differences in threshold doses between some developmental outcomes were observed.

 Conclusion: In the first 6 months of life, increases were observed in tummy time preference as well as tummy time bout frequent and length. This finding may explain why the optimal amount of tummy time needed for more advanced development appeared to increase with age.

What is Known:

• Tummy time is a type of physical activity in infancy. International and national guidelines recommend at least 30 minutes of tummy time per day for infants who are not yet mobile.

What is New:

• In the first six months of life, preference for tummy time as well as tummy time bout frequency and length increased.

• Tummy time duration had dose-response associations with several development outcomes, and the optimal amount of tummy time needed for more advanced development appeared to increase with age.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Abbreviations

ASQ-3:

Ages and Stages Questionnaire

B:

Unstandardized beta coefficient

CI:

Confidence interval

References

  1. Carson V et al (2017) Systematic review of the relationships between physical activity and health indicators in the early years (0–4 years). BMC Public Health 17(5):33–63

    Google Scholar 

  2. Tremblay MS et al (2017) Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines for the early years (0–4 years): an integration of physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep. BMC Public Health 17(5):1–32

    Google Scholar 

  3. World Health Organization (2019) Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. World Health Organization. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664. License: CC BY-NC-SA 3.0 IGO

  4. Carmeli E et al (2009) Preferred sleep position and gross motor achievement in early infancy. Eur J Pediatr 168(6):711

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Rocha NACF, Tudella E (2008) The influence of lying positions and postural control on hand–mouth and hand–hand behaviors in 0–4-month-old infants. Infant Behav Dev 31(1):107–114

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Majnemer A, Barr RG (2006) Association between sleep position and early motor developmentJ Pediatrics 149(5):623–629.e1

  7. Zhang Z et al (2022) Demographic correlates of movement behaviors in infants: a longitudinal study. J Phys Act Health 1(aop):1–9

  8. Felzer-Kim IT et al (2020) Wakeful prone “tummy time” during infancy: how can we help parents? Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 40(6):651–668

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Hewitt L et al (2020) Tummy time and infant health outcomes: a systematic review. Pediatrics 145(6)

  10. Koren A et al (2019) Examining childhood obesity from infancy: the relationship between tummy time, infant BMI-z, weight gain, and motor development—an exploratory study. J Pediatr Health Care 33(1):80–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kuo Y-L et al (2008) The influence of wakeful prone positioning on motor development during the early life. J Dev Behav Pediatr 29(5):367–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. De Kegel A et al (2013) New reference values must be established for the Alberta Infant Motor Scales for accurate identification of infants at risk for motor developmental delay in Flanders. Child Care Health Dev 39(2):260–267

  13. Dudek-Shriber L, Zelazny S (2007) The effects of prone positioning on the quality and acquisition of developmental milestones in four-month-old infants. Pediatr Phys Ther 19(1):48–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Carson V et al (2022) Longitudinal associations between infant movement behaviours and development. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 19(1):1–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Hesketh KD et al (2015) Prevalence and stability of active play, restricted movement and television viewing in infants. Early Child Dev Care 185(6):883–894

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Zhang Z et al (2021) Validity of an infant tummy time questionnaire and time-use diary against the GENEActiv accelerometerMeas Phys Educ Exerc Sci 1–12

  17. Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112(1):155

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Van Der Ploeg HP et al (2010) Advances in population surveillance for physical activity and sedentary behavior: reliability and validity of time use surveys. Am J Epidemiol 172(10):1199–1206

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Squires J, Bricker DD, Twombly E (2009) Ages & stages questionnaires. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes

  20. Gollenberg AL et al (2010) Concurrent validity of the parent‐completed ages and stages questionnaires, with the Bayley scales of infant development II in a low‐risk sample. Child Care Health Dev 36(4):485–490

  21. Hoaglin DC, Iglewicz B (1987) Fine-tuning some resistant rules for outlier labeling. J Am Stat Assoc 82(400):1147–1149

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Siddicky SF et al (2020) Positioning and baby devices impact infant spinal muscle activity. J Biomech 104:109741

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Zachry AH, Kitzmann KM (2011) Caregiver awareness of prone play recommendations. Am J Occup Ther 65(1):101–105

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Senju A et al (2018) Inability of infants to push up in the prone position and subsequent development. Pediatr Int 60(9):811–819

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Soska KC, Adolph KE (2014) Postural position constrains multimodal object exploration in infants. Infancy 19(2):138–161

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Bernstein NA, Latash ML, Turvey MT (2014) Dexterity and its development. Psychology Press

  27. Hewitt L et al (2019) Objective measurement of tummy time in infants (0–6 months): a validation study. PLoS ONE 14(2):e0210977

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG (2009) Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform 42(2):377–381

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful for all the families that participated in the study. The authors would like to thank April English for her help with the project coordination and Jasmine Rai for her help with time-use diary data entry. Study data were collected and managed using REDCap [28] electronic data capture tools hosted and supported by the Women and Children’s Health Research Institute at the University of Alberta.

Funding

This research was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR), Stollery Children’s Hospital Foundation through the Women and Children’s Health Research Institute, and the Faculty of Kinesiology, Sport, and Recreation at the University of Alberta. The sponsor had no role in the study design; collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; and in writing the manuscript. VC is supported by a CIHR New Investigator Salary Award and a Killam Accelerator Research Award.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ZZ analyzed the data, interpreted the results, and drafted the manuscript. MP helped with the recruitment and data collection. KH and LP made contributions to the conception and design of the study. VC conceived and designed the study, secured funding, and oversaw the study conduct, data analysis, and the interpretation of the results. All the authors critically revised the paper for important intellectual content. All the authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Valerie Carson.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

Ethics approval was obtained from the University of Alberta Research Ethics Board.

Consent to participate

Written informed consent was obtained from all participating parents.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Communicated by Peter de Winter

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhang, Z., Predy, M., Hesketh, K.D. et al. Characteristics of tummy time and dose-response relationships with development in infants. Eur J Pediatr 182, 113–121 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-022-04647-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-022-04647-w

Keywords

Navigation