Skip to main content
Log in

Individual differences in dimensions of mind wandering: the mediating role of emotional valence and intentionality

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Correction to this article was published on 09 September 2022

This article has been updated

Abstract

Individual differences in executive control ability reliably show that those with greater executive control report fewer instances of mind wandering during moderately demanding tasks. However, these findings have been limited in that they often treated mind wandering as a variable that collapsed across a variety of thought categories or dimensions. We suggest that two dimensions of mind wandering, intentionality and emotional valence, may be differential related to individual difference in executive control ability. The present study examined this using multiple measures of working memory capacity and attentional control while measuring emotional valence and intentionality of mind wandering during a single sustained attention task. Non-cognitive predictors of mind wandering were also measured. Overall, the results suggest that both working memory capacity and attention control are significant predictors of mind wandering propensity, replicating previous findings. However, the dimensions of emotional valence and intentionality suggested that this finding was not consistent across all types of thought reports. The current findings provide support for the view that it is critical to consider these two dimensions, among other important dimensions, of mind wandering to have a more complete understanding of individual differences in mind wandering.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

Notes

  1. The current study included multiple thought probe responses related to different categories of negative task-unrelated thoughts due to a subsequent writing manipulation designed to explore the impact of a variety of negative moods. For the purpose of the current paper, these negatively valenced TUT response options are collapsed since this data was collected before any mood manipulation.

  2. Due to skewness and kurtosis for positive TUT rates, this variable was transformed to correct for skewness and kurtosis. Following the transformation, all analyses were conducted with the transformed variable. No differences were observed between analyses with the raw and the transformed data. As a result, analysis with the raw data are reported for ease of interpretation.

References

  • Anderson, T., Petranker, R., Lin, H., & Farb, N. A. S. (2021). The metronome response task for measuring mind wandering: Replication attempt and extension of three studies by Seli et al. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 83, 315–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., Smith, G. T., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baird, B., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the future: Autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1604–1611.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Banks, J. B., Jha, A. P., Hood, A. V. B., Goller, H. G., & Craig, L. L. (2019). Reducing the TUTs that hurt: The impact of a brief mindfulness induction on emotionally valenced mind wandering. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 31(8), 785–799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks, J. B., Welhaf, M. S., Hood, A. V. B., Boals, A., & Tartar, J. L. (2016). Examining the role of emotional valence of mind wandering: All mind wandering is not equal. Consciousness and Cognition, 43, 167–176.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, M., & Sackur, J. (2013). Mind wandering at the fingertips: Automatic parsing of subjective states based on response time variability. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 573.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cheyne, A. J., Solman, G. J., Carriere, J. S., & Smilek, D. (2009). Anatomy of an error: A bidirectional state model of task engagement/disengagement and attention-related errors. Cognition, 111(1), 98–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, A.R.A., Kane, M.J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D.Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R.W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methological review and user's guide. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 769–786.

  • Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2009). Harnessing the wandering mind: The role of perceptual load. Cognition, 111(3), 345–355.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 4(1), 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goller, H. G., Banks, J. B., & Meier, M. (2020). An individual differences investigation of the relations among life event stress, working memory capacity, and mind wandering: A preregistered replication-extension study. Memory & Cognition, 48, 759–771.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grodsky, A., & Giambra, L. M. (1990). The consistency across vigilance and reading tasks of individual differences in the occurrence of task-unrelated and task-related images and thoughts. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 10(1), 39–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonkman, L. M., Markus, C. R., Franklin, M. S., & van Dalfsen, J. H. (2017). Mind wandering during attention performance: Effects of ADHD-inattention symptomatology, negative mood, ruminative response style and working memory capacity. PLoS ONE, 12(7), e0181213.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ju, Y. J., & Lien, Y. W. (2018). Who is prone to wander and when? Examining an integrative effect of working memory capacity and mindfulness trait on mind wandering under different task loads. Consciousness and Cognition, 63, 1–10.

  • Kane, M. J., Smeekens, B. A., Meier, M., Welhaf, M., & Phillips, N. (2021). Testing the construct validity of competing measurement approaches to probed mind-wandering reports. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/te9yc.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18(7), 614–621.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Gross, G. M., Chun, C. A., Smeekens, B. A., Meier, M. E., Silvia, P. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2017). For whom the mind wandering, and when, varies across laboratory and daily-life settings. Psychological Science, 28, 1271–1289.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Meier, M. E., Smeekens, B. A., Gross, G. M., Chun, C. A., Silvia, P. J., & Kwapil, T. R. (2016). Individual differences in the executive control of attention, memory, and thought, and their associations with schizotypy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 1017–1048.

    Google Scholar 

  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1999). Thought flow: Properties and mechanisms underlying shifts in content. In J. A. Singer & P. Salovey (Eds.), At play in the fields of consciousness: Essays in honor of Jerome L. Singer (pp. 29–50). Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (2009). Daydreaming and fantasizing: Thought flow and motivation. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation (pp. 225–239). Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knowles, D., & Tay, R. (2002). Driver inattention: More risky than the fatal four? Proceedings of the 2002 Road Safety Research, Policing and Education Conference, Adelaide, SA, 377–392.

  • Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261–273.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, D. B., Smallwood, J., & Davidson, R. J. (2012). The persistence of thought: Evidence for a role of working memory in the maintenance of task-unrelated thinking. Psychological Science, 23(4), 375–380.

  • Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., Yang, L.-X., & Ecker, U. K. H. (2010). A working memory test battery for MATLAB. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 571–585.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 309–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcusson-Clavertz, D., Cardeña, E., & Terhune, D. B. (2016). Daydreaming style moderates the relation between working memory and mind wandering: Integrating two hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 42(3), 451–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcusson-Clavertz, D., Kjell, O. N. E., Kim, J., Persson, S. D., & Cardeña, E. (2020). Sad mood and poor sleep are related to task-unrelated thoughts and experience of diminished cognitive control. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 8940.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 196–204.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin., 136, 188–197.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012a). Drifting from slow to “D’oh!”: Working memory capacity and mind wandering predict extreme reaction times and executive control errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 525–549.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012b). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 302–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meier, M. E. (2018). Can research participants comment authoritatively on the validity of their self-reports of mind wandering and task engagement? A replication and extension of Seli, Jonker, Cheyne, Cortes, and Smilek (2015). Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44(10), 1567–1585.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, M. E. (2019). Is there a positive association between working memory capacity and mind wandering in a low-demand breathing task? A preregistered replication of a study by Levinson, Smallwood, and Davidson (2012). Psychological Science, 30(5), 789–797.

  • Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., Franklin, M. S., Chin, J. M., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2012a). The role of mind-wandering in measurements of general aptitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 788–798.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012b). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12, 442–448.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poerio, G. L., Totterdell, P., & Miles, E. (2013). Mind-wandering and negative mood: Does one thing really lead to another? Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 1412–1421.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Randall, J. G., Oswald, F. L., & Beier, M. E. (2014). Mind-wandering, cognition, and performance: a theory-driven meta-analysis of attention regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1411.

  • Redick, T. S., Broadway, J. M., Meier, M. E., Kuriakose, P. S., Unsworth, N., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Measuring working memory capacity with automated complex span tasks. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 28, 164–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robison, M. K., Miller, A. L., & Unsworth, N. (2020). A multi-faceted approach to understanding individual differences in mind wandering. Cognition, 198, 104078.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robison, M. K., Gath, K. I., & Unsworth, N. (2017). The neurotic wandering mind: An individual differences investigation of neuroticism, mind-wandering, and executive control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(4), 649–663.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robison, M. K., & Unsworth, N. (2018). Cognitive and contextual correlates of spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 85–98.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rummel, J., & Boywitt, C. D. (2014). Controlling the stream of thought: Working memory capacity predicts adjustment of mind-wandering to situational demands. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1309–1315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., & Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 8, 23–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A. (2004). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(7), 319–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering minds and wavering rhythms: linking mind wandering and behavioral variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39(1), 1–5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2014). Not all mind wandering is created equal: Dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 79(5), 750–758.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., Xu, M., Purdon, C., & Smilek, D. (2015a). Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: Implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(5), 1417–1425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Smallwood, J., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2015b). On the relation of mind wandering and ADHD symptomatology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(3), 629–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Maillet, D., Smilek, D., Oakman, J. M., & Schacter, D. L. (2017a). Cognitive aging and the distinction between intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychology and Aging, 32(4), 315–324.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Ralph, B. C. W., Konishi, M., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2017b). What did you have in mind? Examining the content of intentional and unintentional types of mind wandering? Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 149–156.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Ralph, B. C., Risko, E. F., Schooler, J. W., Schacter, D. L., & Smilek, D. (2017c). Intentionality and meta-awareness of mind wandering: Are they one and the same, or distinct dimensions? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(6), 1808–1818.

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2016a). On the necessity of distinguishing between unintentional and intentional mind wandering. Psychological Science, 27(5), 685–691.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016b). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 605–617.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D., Schooler, J. W., & Smilek, D. (2018a). Mind-wandering as a natural kind: A family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(6), 479–490.

  • Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Metzinger, T., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D., Smilek, D. (2018b). The family-resemblances framework for mind-wandering remains well clad. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(11), 959–961.

  • Seli, P., Carriere, J. S., Wammes, J. D., Risko, E. F., Schacter, D. L., & Smilek, D. (2018c). On the clock: Evidence for the rapid and strategic modulation of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 29(8), 1247–1256.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind. Memory and Cognition, 36, 1144–1150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., O’Connor, R. C., & Heim, D. (2005). Rumination, dysphoria, and subjective experience. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 24(4), 355–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946.

  • Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 441.

  • Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., & Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods, wandering minds: negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion, 9(2), 271.

  • Smeekens, B. A., & Kane, M. J. (2016). Working memory capacity, mind wandering, and creative cognition: An individual-differences investigation into the benefits of controlled versus spontaneous thought. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(4), 389–415.

  • Soemer, A., & Schiefele, U. (2019). Text difficulty, topic interest, and mind wandering during reading. Learning and Instruction, 61, 12–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soemer, A., & Schiefele, U. (2020). Working memory capacity and (in)voluntary mind wandering. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 27, 758–767.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maj, M., Van der Linden, M., & D’Argembeau, A. (2011). Mind-wandering: Phenomenology and function as assessed with a novel experience sampling method. Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 370–381.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics. Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Imai, K., & Keele, L. (2014). Mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 59(5), 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., & McMillan, B. D. (2014). Similarities and differences between mind-wandering and external distraction: A latent variable analysis of lapses of attention and their relation to cognitive abilities. Acta Psychologica, 150, 14–25.

  • Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2016). The influence of lapses of attention on working memory capacity. Memory and Cognition, 44, 188–196.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Vugt, M. K., & Broers, N. (2016). Self-reported stickiness of mind-wandering affects task performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wammes, J. D., Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., Boucher, P. O., & Smilek, D. (2016). Mind wandering during lectures II: Relation to academic performance. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 2, 33–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, Y. (2018). Mind-wandering, how do I measure thee with probes? Let me count the ways. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 642–661.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welhaf, M. S., Smeekens, B. A., Gazzia, N. C., Perkins, J. B., Silvia, P. J., Meier, M. E., Kwapil, T. R., & Kane, M. J. (2020). An exploratory analysis of individual differences in mind wandering content and consistency. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7, 103–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welz, A., Reinhard, I., Alpers, G. W., & Kuehner, C. (2018). Happy thoughts: Mind wandering affects mood in daily life. Mindfulness, 9(1), 332–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zanesco, A. P., Denkova, E., Witkin, J. E., & Jha, A. P. (2020). Experience sampling of the degree of mind wandering distinguishes hidden attentional states. Cognition, 2, 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zavagnin, M., Borella, E., & De Beni, R. (2014). When the mind wanders: Age-related differences between young and older adults. Acta Psychologica, 145, 54–64.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan B. Banks.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix A

Appendix A

See Fig. 

Fig. 9
figure 9

Trial scheme for updating counter task

9.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Banks, J.B., Welhaf, M.S. Individual differences in dimensions of mind wandering: the mediating role of emotional valence and intentionality. Psychological Research 86, 1495–1517 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01579-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01579-2

Navigation