Skip to main content
Log in

Spontaneous Theory of Mind is reduced for nonhuman-like agents as compared to human-like agents

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theory of Mind research has shown that we spontaneously take into account other’s beliefs. In the current study, we investigate, with a spontaneous Theory of Mind (ToM) task, if this belief representation also applies to nonhuman-like agents. In a series of three experiments, we show here that we do not spontaneously take into account beliefs of nonhuman-like others, or at least we do it to a lesser extent than for human and human-like agents. Further, the experience we have with the other agent, in our case a dog, does not modulate spontaneous ToM: the same pattern of results was obtained when dog owners and no owners were compared. However, when more attention was attracted to the dog behavior, participants’ behavior was influenced by the beliefs of the dog. In sum, spontaneous belief representation seems to be primarily restricted to human and human-like agents, but can be facilitated when more attention is drawn to a nonhuman-like agent.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bardi, L., Desmet, C., Nijhof, A., Wiersema, J. R., & Brass, M. (2017a). Brain activation for spontaneous and explicit false belief tasks overlaps: new fMRI evidence on belief processing and violation of expectation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 391–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bardi, L., Six, P., & Brass, M. (2017b). Repetitive TMS of the temporo-parietal junction disrupts participant’s expectations in a spontaneous theory of mind task. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12, 1775–1782.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bukowski, H., Hietanen, J. K., & Samson, D. (2015). From gaze cueing to perspective taking: Revisiting the claim that we automatically compute where or what other people are looking at. Visual Cognition, 23(8), 1020–1042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F., Happé, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: a functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. NeuroImage, 12(3), 314–325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deschrijver, E., Bardi, L., Wiersema, J. R., & Brass, M. (2016). Behavioral measures of implicit theory of mind in adults with high functioning autism. Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Creating social connection through inferential reproduction—Loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19, 114–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic Imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 463–483.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, A. C., & Bekoff, M. (2007). Naturalizing anthropomorphism: Behavioral prompts to our humanizing of animals. Anthrozoos, 20, 23–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S. J. (2003). An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13, 522–525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klapper, A., Ramsey, R., Wigboldus, D., & Cross, E. S. (2014). The control of automatic imitation based on bottom-up and top-down cues to animacy: insights from brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(11), 2503–2513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap—Cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—A model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovács, A. M., Teglas, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330, 1830–1834.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kovács, Á. M., Kühn, S., Gergely, G., Csibra, G., & Brass, M. (2014). Are all beliefs equal? Implicit belief attributions recruiting core brain regions of theory of mind. PLoS One, 9(9), e106558.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010b). Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 221–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Longo, M. R., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2009). Attention modulates the specificity of automatic imitation to human actors. Experimental Brain Research, 192(4), 739–744.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muller, B. C. N., Brass, M., Kuhn, S., Tsai, C. C., Nieuwboer, W., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Baaren, R. B. (2011). When Pinocchio acts like a human, a wooden hand becomes embodied. Action co-representation for non-biological agents. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1373–1377.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nijhof, A. D., Brass, M., Bardi, L., & Wiersema, J. R. (2016). Measuring mentalizing ability: A within-subject comparison between an explicit and implicit version of a ball detection task. PLoS One, 11(10), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nijhof, A. D., Brass, M., & Wiersema, J. R. (2017). Spontaneous mentalizing in neurotypicals scoring high versus low on symptomatology of autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Research, 258, 15–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, J., Ong, D. C., Surtees, A. D. R., Xin, Y. J., Williams, S., Saxe, R., & Frank, M. C. (2015). A second look at automatic theory of mind: Reconsidering Kovács, Teglas, and Endress (2010). Psychological Science, 26, 1353–1367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Press, C., Gillmeister, H., & Heyes, C. (2006). Bottom-up, not top-down, modulation of imitation by human and robotic models. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24(8), 2415–2419.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D., Bayliss, A. P., Becker, S. I., & Dux, P. E. (2011). Eye movements reveal sustained implicit processing of others’ mental states. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 433–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., & Dux, P. E. (2017). Current evidence for automatic Theory of Mind processing in adults. Cognition, 162, 27–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, A. R., Forstmann, M., Burgmer, P., Brooks, A. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). Anxious and egocentric: How specific emotions influence perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), 374–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Todd, A. R., & Simpson, A. J. (2016). Anxiety impairs spontaneous perspective calculation: Evidence from a level-1 visual perspective-taking task. Cognition, 156, 88–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsai, C. C., & Brass, M. (2007). Does the human motor system simulate Pinocchio’s actions? Coacting with a human hand versus a wooden hand in a dyadic interaction. Psychological Science, 18, 1058–1062.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiese, E., Wykowska, A., Zwickel, J., & Müller, H. J. (2012). I see what you mean: How attentional selection is shaped by ascribing intentions to others. PloS One, 7, e45391.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by FP7 Marie Sklodowska-Curie Individual Fellowship to Lara Bardi (GRANT 331323-Mirroring and ToM).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lara Bardi.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Author LB declares that she has no conflict of interest. Author CD declares that she has no conflict of interest. Author MB declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (M4V 935 KB)

Supplementary material 2 (M4V 1092 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bardi, L., Desmet, C. & Brass, M. Spontaneous Theory of Mind is reduced for nonhuman-like agents as compared to human-like agents. Psychological Research 83, 1571–1580 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1000-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1000-0

Navigation