Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spaces

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Identifying individuals’ intent through the emotional valence conveyed by their facial expression influences our capacity to approach-avoid these individuals during social interactions. Here, we explore if and how the emotional valence of others’ facial expressiveness modulates peripersonal-action and interpersonal-social spaces. Through Immersive Virtual Reality, participants determined reachability-distance (for peripersonal space) and comfort-distance (for interpersonal space) from male/female virtual confederates exhibiting happy, angry and neutral facial expressions while being approached by (passive-approach) or walking toward (active-approach) them. Results showed an increase of distance when seeing angry rather than happy confederates in both approach conditions of comfort-distance. The effect also appeared in reachability-distance, but only in the passive-approach. Anger prompts avoidant behaviors, and thus an expansion of distance, particularly with a potential violation of near body space by an intruder. Overall, the findings suggest that peripersonal-action space, in comparison with interpersonal-social space, is similarly sensitive to the emotional valence of stimuli. We propose that this similarity could reflect a common adaptive mechanism shared by these spaces, presumably at different degrees, for ensuring self-protection functions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, R. B., Ambady, N., Macrae, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2006). Emotional expressions forecast approach-avoidance behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 179–188. doi:10.1007/s11031-006-9020-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aiello, J. R. (1987). Human Spatial Behavior. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 389–504). New York: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M., & Dean, J. (1965). Eye-contact, distance and affiliation. Sociometry, 28, 289–304. doi:10.2307/2786027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armbrüster, C., Wolter, M., Kuhlen, T., Spijkers, W., & Fimm, B. (2008). Depth Perception in Virtual Reality: Distance Estimations in Peri- and Extrapersonal Space. Cyber Psychology & Behavior, 11, 9–15.

  • Bailenson, J. N., Blascovich, J., Beall, A. C., & Loomis, J. M. (2003). Interpersonal distance in immersive virtual environments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 819–833. doi:10.1177/0146167203029007002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berti, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2000). When far becomes near: Remapping of space by tool use. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 415–420. doi:10.1162/089892900562237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brozzoli, C., Gentile, G., Bergouignan, L., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2013). A shared representation of the space near onself and others in the human premotor cortex. Current Biology, 23, 1764–1768. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(1), 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coello, Y., Bourgeois, J., & Iachini, T. (2012). Embodied perception of reachable space: How do we manage threatening objects? Cognitive Processing, 13, 131–135. doi:10.1007/s10339-012-0470-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coello, Y., & Iachini, T. (2015). Embodied perception of objects and people in space: Towards a unified theoretical framework. In Y. Coello & M. Fischer (Eds.), Foundations of embodied cognition (pp. 198–219). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, S., Balcetis, B., & Dunning, D. (2013). Affective signals of threat increase perceived proximity. Psychological Science, 24, 34–40. doi:10.1177/0956797612446953.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace. doi:10.1080/15294145.2000.10773287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872). The expressions of the emotions in men and animals. London: John Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., Pourtois, G., & Weiskrantz, L. (1999). Non-conscious recognition of affect in the absence of striate cortex. NeuroReport, 16(10), 3759–3763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Vignemont, F., & Iannetti, G. D. (2015). How many peripersonal spaces? Neuropsychologia, 70, 327–334. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • di Pellegrino, G., & Làdavas, E. (2015). Peripersonal space in the brain. Neuropsychologia, 66, 126–133. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.11.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dosey, M. A., & Meisels, M. (1969). Personal space and self-protection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 93–97. doi:10.1037/h0027040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (1999). Facial expressions. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), The handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 301–320). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erle, T. M., & Topolinski, S. (2015). Spatial and empathic perspective-taking correlate on a dispositional level. Social Cognition, 33, 187–210. doi:10.1521/soco.2015.33.3.187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farné, A., Iriki, A., & Ladavas, E. (2005). Shaping multisensory action-space with tools: Evidence from patients with cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia, 43, 238–248. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 396–403. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gessaroli, E., Santelli, E., di Pellegrino, G., & Frassinetti, F. (2013). Personal space regulation in childhood autism spectrum disorders. PLoS One, 8(9), e74959. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0074959.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Graziano, M. S. A., & Cooke, D. F. (2006). Parieto-frontal interactions, personal space, and defensive behavior. Neuropsychologia, 44, 845–859. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1966). The hidden dimension. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayduk, L. A. (1983). Personal space: Where we now stand. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 293–335. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.94.2.293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebl, M. R., & Kleck, R. E. (2002). Virtually interactive: A new paradigm for the analysis of stigma. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 128–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hediger, H. (1955). Studies of psychology and behavior of captive animals in zoos and circuses. London, Butterworth, New York: Criterion books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horstmann, G. (2003). What do facial expression convey: Feeling, states behavioral intentions, or actions requests? Emotion, 32, 150–166. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.3.2.150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2014). Body space in social interactions: A comparison of reaching and comfort distance in immersive virtual reality. PLoS ONE, 9(11), e111511. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0111511.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Iachini, T., Coello, Y., Frassinetti, F., Senese, V. P., Galante, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2016). Peripersonal and interpersonal space in virtual and real environments: Effects of gender and age. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 45, 154–164. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.01.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iachini, T., Pagliaro, S., & Ruggiero, G. (2015a). Near or far? It depends on my impression: Moral information and spatial behavior in virtual interactions. Acta Psychologica, 161, 131–136. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iachini, T., Ruggiero, G., Ruotolo, F., Schiano di Cola, A., & Senese, V. P. (2015b). The influence of anxiety and personality factors on comfort and reachability space: A correlational study. Cognitive Processing, 16, 255–258. doi:10.1007/s10339-015-0717-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jabbi, M., & Keysers, C. (2008). Inferior frontal gyrus activity triggers anterior insula response to emotional facial expressions. Emotion, 8, 775–780. doi:10.1037/a0014194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Ekman, P., Gonzaga, G. C., & Beer, J. (2003). Facial expression of emotion. In D. J. Richard, K. R. Klaus, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences. Series in affective science (pp. 415–432). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, D. P., Gläscher, J., Tyszka, J. M., & Adolphs, R. (2009). Personal space regulation by the human amygdala. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 1226–1227. doi:10.1038/nn.2381.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20(3), 165–182. doi:10.1007/BF02281954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lampton, D. R., McDonald, D. P., Singer, M., & Bliss, J. P. (1995). Distance estimation in virtual environments. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 39th Annual Meeting, 1268–1272.

  • LeDoux, J. (2003). The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 23(4), 727–738. doi:10.1023/A:1025048802629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, D. M. (2009). The space between us: A neurophilosophical framework for the investigation of human interpersonal space. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 297–304. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loomis, J. M., Blaskovich, J. J., & Beall, A. C. (1999). Immersive virtual environment technology as a basic research tool in psychology. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 557–564.

  • Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces—KDEF. CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, ISBN 91-630-7164-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/t27732-000

  • Marsh, A. A., Ambady, N., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach—and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion, 5(1), 119–124. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nomura, M., Ohira, H., Haneda, K., Iidaka, T., Sadato, N., Okada, T., et al. (2004). Functional association of the amygdala and ventral prefrontal cortex during cognitive evaluation of facial expressions primed by masked angry faces: An event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 21, 352–363. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A. (1987). The psychophysiology of emotion: An evolutionary-cognitive perspective. In P. K. Ackles, J. R. Jennings, & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.), Advances in psychophysiology (Vol. 2, pp. 79–127). Greenwich: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Patané, I., Iachini, T., Farnè, A., & Frassinetti, F. (2016). Disentangling action from social space: Tool-use differently shapes the space around us. PLoS One, 11(5), e0154247. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0154247.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Quesque, F., Ruggiero, G., Mouta, S., Santos, J., Iachini, T., & Coello, Y. (2016). Keeping you at arm’s length: Modifying peripersonal space influences interpersonal distance. Psychological Research,. doi:10.1007/s00426-016-0782-1.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1997). The space around us. Science, 277, 190–191. doi:10.1126/science.277.5323.190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rolland, J. P., Gibson, W., & Ariely, D. (1995). Towards quantifying depth and size perception in virtual environments. Presence, 4, 24–49.

  • Seidel, E. M., Habel, U., Kirschner, M., Gur, R. C., & Derntl, B. (2010). The impact of facial emotional expressions on behavioral tendencies in women and men. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 500–507. doi:10.1037/a0018169.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Siegman, A. W., & Feldstein, S. (2014). Nonverbal behavior and communication. Abingdon: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater, M. (2009). Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 3549–3557. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taffou, M., & Viaud-Delmon, I. (2014). Cynophobic fear adaptively extends peri-personal space. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 122. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Pantelidou, G., Rebacz, P., Västfjäll, D., & Tsakiris, M. (2011). I-Space: The effects of emotional valence and source of music on interpersonal distance. PLoS ONE, 6(10), e26083. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026083.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Teneggi, C., Canzoneri, E., di Pellegrino, G., & Serino, A. (2013). Social modulation of peripersonal space boundaries. Current Biology, 23, 406–411. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uzzell, D., & Horne, N. (2006). The influence of biological sex, sexuality and gender role on interpersonal distance. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 579–597. doi:10.1348/014466605x58384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Dantzig, S., Pecher, D., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Approach and avoidance as action effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(9), 1298–1306. doi:10.1080/17470210802027987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P., & Pourtois, G. (2007). Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotions face perception: Evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia, 45, 174–194. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gennaro Ruggiero.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This study was not supported by grant or funding.

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in the present study involving healthy participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

 Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ruggiero, G., Frassinetti, F., Coello, Y. et al. The effect of facial expressions on peripersonal and interpersonal spaces. Psychological Research 81, 1232–1240 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0806-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0806-x

Keywords

Navigation