Skip to main content
Log in

Effect of combined motor and spatial cues on mathematical reasoning: a polarity correspondence account

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined whether combined motor or spatial polarities could influence accuracy in two mathematical operations. Four experiments were conducted and showed that, when two corresponding polarities were activated, accuracy in multiplicative operations was greater than when non-corresponding polarities were activated, whereas no effect was found for additive operations. These results were established with motor cues (Left/Right and Arm Extension/Flexion, as behavioral approach-avoidance tendencies) and perceptual spatial cues (Left/Right and DOWN/UP cues). A polarity correspondence effect was established and proposed for multiplication. A combination of polarities was associated with a corresponding combination of numerical digits, assessed with mathematical operations, such as multiplication.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, N. H. (1981). Foundations of Information Integration Theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. H. (1982). Methods for Information Integration Theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. H. (1996). A functional theory of cognition. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. H. (2008). Unified social cognition. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. H. (2013). Unified psychology based on three laws of information integration. Review of General Psychology, 17, 125–132. doi:10.1037/a0032921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anelli, F., Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2014). Walking boosts your performance in making additions and subtractions. Frontiers in Psychology Cognition, 5, 1459. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badets, A., Koch, I., & Toussaint, L. (2013). Role of an ideomotor mechanism in number processing. Experimental Psychology, 60, 34–43. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Badets, A., & Pesenti, M. (2011). Finger-number interaction: An ideomotor account. Experimental Psychology, 58, 287–292. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bae, G. Y., Choi, J. M., Cho, Y. S., & Proctor, R. W. (2009). Transfer of magnitude and spatial mappings to the SNARC effect for parity judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 35, 1506–1521. doi:10.1080/17470210802303883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. I. D., & Austin, S. (2002). Effects of response time deadlines on adults’ strategy choices for simple addition. Memory and Cognition, 30, 988–994. doi:10.3758/BF03195782.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, R. A., Avraamides, M. N., Cary, M., & Strasberg, S. (2007). What do the hands externalize in simple arithmetic? Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 33, 747–756. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cretenet, J., & Dru, V. (2004). The influence of unilateral and bilateral arm flexion vs. extension on judgments: An exploratory case of a motor congruence hypothesis. Emotion, 4, 282–294. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.4.3.282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cretenet, J., & Dru, V. (2009). Influence of peripheral and motivational cues on rigid-flexible functioning: Perceptual, behavioral, and cognitive aspects. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 138, 201–217. doi:10.1037/a0015379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cretenet, J., Mullet, E., & Dru, V. (2015). Motor and cognitive integration: Effect of bilateral behaviors on judgment. Acta Psychologica, 161, 64–72. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.08.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 122, 371–396. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Didino, D., Lombardi, L., & Vespignani, F. (2014). Operand-order effect in multiplication and addition: The long-term effects of reorganization process and acquisition sequence. Experimental Psychology, 61, 470–479. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, J. A., Deets, J. K., & Banfert, A. (2001). The representations of the arithmetic operations include functional relationships. Memory and Cognition, 29, 462–477. doi:10.3758/bf03196397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dru, V., & Cretenet, J. (2008). Influence of unilateral motor behaviors on the judgment of valenced stimuli. Cortex, 44, 717–727. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2006.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eikmeier, V., Hoppe, D., & Ulrich, R. (2015). Responde mode does not modulate the space-time congruency effect: Evidence for a space-time mapping at a conceptual level. Acta Psychologica, 156, 162–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2012). A hierarchical view of grounded, embodied, and situated numerical cognition. Cognitive Processing, 13, 161–164. doi:10.1007/s10339-012-0477-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gevers, W., Lammertyn, J., Notebaert, W., Verguts, T., & Fias, W. (2006). Automatic response activation of implicit spatial information: Evidence from SNARC effect. Acta Psychologica, 122, 221–233. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, K. J., & Lourenco, S. F. (2012). Orienting numbers in mental space: Horizontal organization trumps vertical. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1044–1051. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.685079.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Memory and Cognition, 32, 662–673. doi:10.3758/BF03195857.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirk, E. P., & Ashcraft, M. H. (2001). Telling stories: The perils and promise of using verbal reports to study math strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 27, 157–175. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knops, A., Viarouge, A., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Dynamic representations underlying symbolic and nonsymbolic calculation: Evidence from the operational momentum effect. Attention Perception and Psychophysics, 71, 803–821. doi:10.3758/APP.71.4.803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krist, H., Fieberg, E. L., & Wilkening, F. (1993). Intuitive physics in action and judgment: The development of knowledge about projectile motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 19, 952–966. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.4.952.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakens, D. (2012). Polarity correspondence in metaphor congruency effects: Structural overlap predicts categorization times for bi-polar concepts presented in vertical space. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 38, 726–736. doi:10.1037/a0024955.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2000). Where Mathematics Comes From: How the Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics Into Being. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lugli, L., Baroni, G., Anelli, F., Borghi, A. M., & Nicoletti, R. (2013). Counting is easier while experiencing a congruent motion. PLoS One, 8, e64500. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0064500.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lynott, D., & Coventry, K. R. (2014). On the ups and downs of emotion: Testing between conceptual-metaphor and polarity accounts of emotional valence-spatial location interactions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21, 218–226. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0481-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2014). Attentional bias induced by solving simple and complex addition and subtraction problems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1414–1526. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.903985.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, J. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Emotion as motion: Asymmetries in approach and avoidant actions. Psychological Science, 18, 1113–1119. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02033.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCrink, K., Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2007). Moving along the number line: Operational momentum in nonsymbolic arithmetic. Perception and Psychophysics, 69, 1324–1333. doi:10.3758/BF03192949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mullet, E., Cretenet, J., & Dru, V. (2014). Motor influences on judgment: Motor and cognitive integration. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 69–91. doi:10.1111/bjop.12022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R., Rasmussen, C., Sweeney, G., & Wawro, M. (2011). When the classroom floor becomes the complex plane: Addition and multiplication as ways of bodily navigation. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21, 287–323. doi:10.1080/10508406.2011.611445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porac, C., & Cohen, S. (1981). Lateral Preferences and Human Behavior. New York: Springer. doi:10.2307/1422823.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416–442. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R. W., & Xiong, A. (2015). Polarity correspondence as a general compatibility principle. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 446–451. doi:10.1177/0963721415607305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2009). Why do gestures matter? Sensuous cognition and the palpability of mathematical meanings. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70, 111–126. doi:10.1007/s10649-008-9127-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., Wurtz, P., Knapstad, M., & Lervik, L. V. (2010). Polarity correspondence in comparative number magnitude judgments. Psychonomic and Bulletin Review, 17, 219–223. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.2.219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rulences-Pâques, P., & Mullet, E. (1998). Area judgment from with and height information: The case of the rectangle. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 69, 22–48. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russo, J. E., Johnson, E. J., & Stephens, D. L. (1989). The validity of verbal protocols. Memory and Cognition, 17, 759–769.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Santiago, J., & Lakens, D. (2014). Can conceptual congruency effects between number, time, and space be accounted for by polarity correspondence? Acta Psychologica,. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.09.016.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semin, G. R., & Smith, E. R. (2008). Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Topolinski, S., & Reber, R. (2010). Immediate truth-temporal contiguity between a cognitive problem and its solution determines experienced veracity of the solution. Cognition, 114, 117–122. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tschentscher, N., Hauk, O., Fischer, M. H., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). You can count on the motor system: Finger counting habits modulate motor cortex activation evoked by numbers. Neuroimage, 59, 3139–3148. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verguts, T., Fias, W., & Stevens, M. (2005). A model of exact small number representation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 66–80. doi:10.3758/BF03196349.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiemers, M., Bekkering, H., & Lindemann, O. (2014). Spatial interferences in mental arithmetic: Evidence from the motion-arithmetic compatibility effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1557–1570. doi:10.1080/17470218.2014.889180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank R. Sanz, Jeremy Hébert, Jéremie Vic-Accariès and Nadine Clémencet for their technical assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vincent Dru.

Ethics declarations

Funding

No funding was associated with this research.

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

H. Verselder, S. Freddi, and V. Dru contributed equally to this research.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Verselder, H., Freddi, S. & Dru, V. Effect of combined motor and spatial cues on mathematical reasoning: a polarity correspondence account. Psychological Research 81, 910–924 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0799-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0799-5

Keywords

Navigation