Skip to main content
Log in

Trajectory deviations in spatial compatibility tasks with peripheral and central stimuli

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It is widely held that the spatial compatibility effect emerges because the irrelevant spatial dimension of the target stimulus activates a response simultaneous to the activation of a response to the relevant stimulus dimension. The non-target response facilitates response planning on compatible trials, but interferes with response planning on incompatible trials. In support of this hypothesis, the trajectories of aiming movements executed on incompatible trials deviate in the direction of the stimulus location. These deviations are thought to occur because the characteristics of the simultaneously active target and non-target responses merge. Previously, target stimuli were presented at the target locations leaving open the possibility that the response activation and subsequent deviations were dependent on the stimulus-driven attentional capture associated with the dynamic change of stimulus onset. The present research was conducted to determine if attention capturing events at the target locations were necessary for the movement deviations to emerge by investigating if trajectory deviations are also observed when the spatial dimension is presented centrally. Participants in the Central condition aimed to left and right target locations based on the color of a ring worn on a finger that pointed toward the response locations. Similar to results from a Peripheral condition, trajectory deviations were observed in the Central condition. Although it is unclear if the deviations in the Central condition occurred because of attentional shifts driven by the cue or response preplanning, these results demonstrate that dynamic changes at target locations are sufficient, but not necessary, to generate trajectory deviations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Atsunori, A., & Katsumi, W. (2009). What is special about the index finger? The index finger advantage in manipulating reflexive attentional shift. Japanese Psychological Research, 51, 258–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Böckler, A., van der Wel, R. P., & Welsh, T. N. (2014). Catching eyes: effects of social and non-social cues on attention capture. Psychological Science, 25, 720–727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buetti, S., & Kerzel, D. (2009). Conflicts during response selection affect response programming: reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 816–834.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisek, P., & Kalaska, J. F. (2002). Simultaneous encoding of multiple potential reach directions in dorsal premotor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 87, 1149–1154.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cisek, P., & Kalaska, J. F. (2005). Neural correlates of reaching decisions in dorsal premotor cortex: specification of multiple direction choices and final selection of action. Neuron, 45, 801–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, R., Liang, C. C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 18, 1030–1044.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 490–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: visual attention, social cognition, and individual differences. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 694–724.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos, A. P. (1990). Neurophysiology of reaching. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Attention and performance XIII (pp. 227–263). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Pratt, J., Colzato, L., & Godijn, R. (2001). Symbolic control of visual attention. Psychological Science, 12, 360–365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. Long & A. Baddley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawerence Erlbaum Assoc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khun, G., & Kingstone, A. (2009). Look away! Eyes and arrows engage oculomotor responses automatically. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71, 314–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langton, S. R. H., & Bruce, V. (1999). Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others. Visual Cognition, 6, 541–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. Bowhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B.M. (1994). Space and selective attention. In C. Umilta & M Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV (pp. 231–265). Cambridge, MA: MIT.

  • Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., & Yoshikawa, S. (2010). Automatic attentional shifts by gaze, gestures, and symbols. Psychologia, 53, 27–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scherbaum, S., Dshemuchadse, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2010). How decisions evolve: the temporal dynamics of action selection. Cognition, 115, 407–416.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition, 88, B11–B21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus–response compatibility: An integrated perspective (pp. 31–86). North-Holland: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Song, J.-H., & Nakayama, K. (2009). Hidden cognitive states revealed in choice reaching tasks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 360–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S., Howard, L., & Jackson, S. (1997). Selective reaching to grasp: evidence for distractor interference effects. Visual Cognition, 4, 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., & Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: evidence for action-centred attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 891–905.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Wel, R. P. R. D., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2013). Action perception from a common coding perspective. In K. Johnson & M. Shiffrar (Eds.), People watching: Social, perceptual, and neurophysiological studies of body perception (pp. 101–120). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N. (2011). The relationship between attentional capture and deviations in movement trajectories in a selective reaching task. Acta Psychologica, 137, 300–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2004). Movement trajectories in the presence of a distracting stimulus: evidence for a response activation model of selective reaching. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1031–1057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Elliott, D., & Weeks, D. J. (1999). Hand deviations towards distractors: evidence for response competition. Experimental Brain Research, 127, 207–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., & Weeks, D. J. (2010). Visual selective attention and action. In D. Elliott & M. A. Khan (Eds.), Vision and goal-directed movement: Neurobehavioral perspectives (pp. 39–58). Champaign Illinois: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was funded through grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, the Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation, and Canada Foundation for Innovation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Timothy N. Welsh.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Welsh, T.N., Pacione, S.M., Neyedli, H.F. et al. Trajectory deviations in spatial compatibility tasks with peripheral and central stimuli. Psychological Research 79, 650–657 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0597-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0597-x

Keywords

Navigation