Skip to main content
Log in

Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied?

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Generally, so-called control processes are thought to be necessary when we must perform one out of several competing actions. Some examples include performance of a less well-practiced action instead of a well-practiced one (prepotency); learning a new action (novelty); and rapidly switching from one action to another (task-switching). While it certainly is difficult to perform the desired action in these circumstances, it is less clear that a separate set of processes (e.g., control processes) are necessary to explain the observed behavior. Another way to approach the study of control processes is to investigate physiological dependent measures (e.g., electrophysiological or neuroimaging measures). Although these offer another avenue of inquiry into control processes, they have yet to furnish unambiguous evidence that control processes exist. While this might suggest that there are no control processes, it is also possible that our methods are insufficiently sensitive to measure control processes. We have investigated this latter possibility using tasks that are neuroanatomically distinct, though within the same modality (vision). This approach did not yield evidence for a separable set of control processes. However, recent works using a task-switching paradigm in which subjects switch between a visual and an auditory task suggest that switching both task and modality may be importantly different than switching task within a given modality. This may represent a way forward in the study of control processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alain, C., Arnott, S. R., Hevenor, S., Graham, S., & Grady, C.L. (2001). “What” and “where” in the human auditory system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(21), 12301–12306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. C. Umilta & Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XI (pp. 107–132). Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Allport, D. A., & Wylie, G. R. (2000). Task-switching, stimulus-response bindings, and negative priming. In S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and Performance XVIII (pp. 35–70). Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Archibald, S. J., Mateer, C. A., & Kerns, K. A. (2001). Utilization behavior: Clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychology Review, 11(3), 117–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arrington, C. M., & Logan, G. D. (2004). The cost of a voluntary task switch. Psychological Science, 15(9), 610–615.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arrington, C. M., Weaver, S. M., & Pauker, R. L. (2010). Stimulus-based priming of task choice during voluntary task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1060–1067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(1), 5–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., Ruge, H., Rubin, O., Koch, I., Prinz, W., von Cramona, D. Y., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2003). When the same response has different meanings: Recoding the response meaning in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage, 20(2), 1026–1031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. Cerebral Cortex, 12(9), 908–914.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Decomposing components of task preparation with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 609–620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1971). The case against B.F. Skinner. New York: The New York Review of Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Santis, L., Clarke, S., & Murray, M. M. (2007). Automatic and intrinsic auditory “what” and “where” processing in humans revealed by electrical neuroimaging. Cerebral Cortex, 17(1), 9–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeJong, R. (2001). An intention-activation account of residual switch costs. Control of cognitive processes. S. Monsell & J. S. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 357–376). Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Demakis, G. J. (2003). A meta-analytic review of the sensitivity of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to frontal and lateralized frontal brain damage. Neuropsychology, 17(2), 255–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dove, A., Pollmann, S., et al. (2000). Prefrontal cortex activation in task switching: An event-related fMRI study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 9(1), 103–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dreher, J. C., Koechlin, E., Ali, S. O., & Grafman, J. (2002). The roles of timing and task order during task switching. Neuroimage, 17(1), 95–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forstmann, B. U., Brass, M., Koch, I., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Voluntary selection of task sets revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 388–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forstmann, B. U., Wolfensteller, U., Derrfuss, J., Neumann, J., Brass, M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & von Cramon, D. V. (2008). When the choice is ours: Context and agency modulate the neural bases of decision-making. PLoS One, 3(4), e1899.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gurd, J. M., Amunts, K., Weia, P. H., Zafiris, O., Zilles, K., Marshall, J. C., & Fink, G. R. (2002). Posterior parietal cortex is implicated in continuous switching between verbal fluency tasks: An fMRI study with clinical implications. Brain, 125(Pt 5), 1024–1038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haxby, J. V., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Maisog, J. M., Pietrini, P., & Grady, C. L. (1994). “The functional organization of human extrastriate cortex: A PET-rCBF study of selective attention to faces and locations.”. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(11 Pt 1), 6336–6353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1998). Event Files: Evidence for automatic integration of stimulus-response episodes. Visual Cognition, 5(1), 183–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubner, R., Futterer, T., & Steinhauser, M. (2001). On attentional control as a source of residual shift costs: Evidence from two-component task shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 640–653.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, A. R., & Kingstone, A. (2004). Multisensory executive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 55(2), 325–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, I. H., Brooks, D. J., Nixon, P. D., Frackowiak, R. S., & Passingham, R. E. (1994). Motor sequence learning: A study with positron emission tomography. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(6), 3775–3790.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of psychology 89, 5–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaas, J. H., & Hackett, T. A. (2000). Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(22), 11793–11799.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimberg, D. Y., Aguirre, G. K., & D'Esposito, M. (2000). Modulation of task-related neural activity in task-switching: An fMRI study. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 10(1–2), 189–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinsorge, T., & Heuer, H. (1999). Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychological Research, 62(4), 300–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, I. (2003). The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfiguration in task switching. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(2), 488–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(1), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (2007). What it costs to implement a plan: Plan-level and task-level contributions to switch costs. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 591–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010a). The role of preparation and cue-modality in crossmodal task switching. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam), 134(3), 318–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010b). Switching attention between modalities: Further evidence for visual dominance. Psychological Research, 74(3), 255–267.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and Stroop-like interference: Evidence for a continuum of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(1), 126–135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maeder, P. P., Meuli, R. A., Adriani, M., Bellmann, A., Fornari, E., Thiran, J. P., Pittet, A., & Clarke, S. (2001). Distinct pathways involved in sound recognition and localization: A human fMRI study. Neuroimage, 14(4), 802–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., & Bell, T. (2006). On how to be unpredictable: Evidence from the voluntary task-switching paradigm. Psychological Science, 17(9), 774–780.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesulam, M. M. (2000). Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, M. M., De Santis, L., Thut, G., & Wylie, G. R. (2009). The costs of crossing paths and switching tasks between audition and vision. Brain and Cognition, 69(1), 47–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, R., Karayanidis F., Bumak, E., Poboka, D., & Michie, P. T. (2006). ERPs dissociate the effects of switching task sets and task cues. Brain Research, 1095(1), 107–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, R., Karayanidis, F., Poboka, D., Heathcote, A., & Michie, P. T. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory task-switching processes. Psychophysiology, 42(5), 540–554.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Philipp, A. M., Kalinich, C., Koch, I., & Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Mixing costs and switch costs when switching stimulus dimensions in serial predictions. Psychological Research, 72(4), 405–414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2005). Switching of response modalities. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 58(7), 1325–1338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). The integration of task-set components into cognitive task representations. Psychologica Belgica, 50(3&4), 383–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rivier, F., & Clarke, S. (1997). Cytochrome oxidase, acetylcholinesterase, and NADPH-diaphorase staining in human supratemporal and insular cortex: Evidence for multiple auditory areas. Neuroimage, 6(4), 288–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). The cost of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Discussion, 124, 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romanski, L. M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Rauschecker, J. P. (1999). Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2(12), 1131–1136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 763–797.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2010). The target of task switching. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(2), 129–133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sohn, M. H., Ursu, S., Anderson, J. R., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Inaugural article: The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(24), 13448–13453.

  • Stephan, D. N., & Koch, I. (2010). Central cross-talk in task switching: Evidence from manipulating input-output modality compatibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1075–1081.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sylvester, C. Y., Wager, T. D., Lacey, S. C., Hernandez, L., Nichols, T. E., Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (2003). Switching attention and resolving interference: fMRI measures of executive functions. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 357–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toni, I., Krams, M., Turner, R., & Passingham, R. E. (1998). The time course of changes during motor sequence learning: A whole-brain fMRI study. Neuroimage, 8(1), 50–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. D. J. Ingle, M.A. Goodale & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–586). London: The MIT Press.

  • Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, D. A. (2003). Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46(4), 361–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G., & Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs”. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 212–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2003a). Cognitive control processes during an anticipated switch of task. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(3), 667–672.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2003b). Task switching: A high-density electrical mapping study. Neuroimage, 20(4), 2322–2342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2004a). Don’t think of a white bear: An fMRI investigation of the effects of sequential instructional sets on cortical activity in a task-switching paradigm. Human Brain Mapping, 21, 279–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., Foxe, J. J. (2004b). The role of response requirements in task switching: Dissolving the residue. Neuroreport, 15(6), 1079–1087.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G. R., Javitt, D. C., & Foxe, J. J. (2006). Jumping the gun: Is effective preparation contingent upon anticipatory activation in task-relevant neural circuitry? Cerebral Cortex, 16(3), 394–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, G. R., Murray, M. M., & Foxe, J. J. (2009). Distinct neurophysiological mechanisms mediate mixing costs and switch costs. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 105–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N. (2010). Bottom-up influences on voluntary task switching: The elusive homunculus escapes. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(2), 348–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., & Monsell, S. (2003). The effects of recent practice on task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 919–936.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, N., Nystrom, L. E., Aronson, J. A., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). Between-task competition and cognitive control in task switching. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(5), 1429–1438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We wish to acknowledge The Kessler Foundation Research Center (GW and JS) and the Swiss National Science Foundation (Grant #310030B_133136; MM) for the support they have provided.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Glenn R. Wylie.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wylie, G.R., Sumowski, J.F. & Murray, M. Are there control processes, and (if so) can they be studied?. Psychological Research 75, 535–543 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0354-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0354-3

Keywords

Navigation