Skip to main content
Log in

Perception–production relationships and phase correction in synchronization with two-interval rhythms

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments investigated the effects of interval duration ratio on perception of local timing perturbations, accuracy of rhythm production, and phase correction in musicians listening to or tapping in synchrony with cyclically repeated auditory two-interval rhythms. Ratios ranged from simple (1:2) to complex (7:11, 5:13), and from small (5:13 = 0.38) to large (6:7 = 0.86). Rhythm production and perception exhibited similar ratio-dependent biases: rhythms with small ratios were produced with increased ratios, and timing perturbations in these rhythms tended to be harder to detect when they locally increased the ratio than when they reduced it. The opposite held for rhythms with large ratios. This demonstrates a close relation between rhythm perception and production. Unexpectedly, however, the neutral “attractor” was not the simplest ratio (1:2 = 0.50) but a complex ratio near 4:7 (= 0.57). Phase correction in response to perturbations was generally rapid and did not show the ratio-dependent biases observed in rhythm perception and production. Thus, phase correction operates efficiently and autonomously even in synchronization with rhythms exhibiting complex interval ratios.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Dynamic systems approaches to rhythmic bimanual coordination also employ a form of hierarchical interval ratio, namely relative phase (e.g., Haken, Kelso, & Bunz, 1985; Tuller & Kelso, 1989; Yamanishi, Kawato, & Suzuki, 1980). A 1:2 interval ratio implies a 1:3 ratio between the shorter interval and the cycle duration (a normalized relative phase of 0.33). It is well known that in-phase and anti-phase movements are easy to perform accurately, whereas other phase relationships between the hands are more difficult to produce, show greater variability, and are typically distorted in the direction of the closer of the two “attractors,” in-phase and anti-phase. These studies, however, are usually not concerned with the production of musical rhythm, and they often use visual pacing signals (but see Semjen & Ivry, 2001). In bimanual production of auditory two-interval rhythms, in-phase and anti-phase movements both imply isochrony and thus are avoided (i.e., do not function as attractors) unless the two target interval durations are difficult to discriminate.

  2. This was really the primary purpose of this research, at least as we saw it initially. However, because we obtained rhythm production data as a byproduct of the synchronization task that we employed to measure phase correction and then realized the perception task could easily be conducted with the identical materials, we did not want to miss this opportunity to compare rhythm production and perception. Thus, we expanded the scope of the present research.

  3. The Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied to the p value of any effect with more than two levels.

  4. These results would change little if the perception data were analyzed in terms of fitting psychometric functions to the response percentages and comparing their slopes and 50% thresholds. It was clear from inspection of the average response functions that, whenever a change was more difficult to detect, the function had both a shallower slope and a higher threshold, and in some cases it did not reach 50%.

  5. Each PCR function also yielded a measure of the y-axis intercept. This intercept was different from zero in non-isochronous rhythms and corresponded closely to the difference between the mean asynchronies of taps with first and second tones (Fig. 2).

  6. We first attempted to derive separate PCR estimates for negative and positive phase shifts by regression analysis, but the results were highly variable due to the small number of data points. Therefore, separate PCRs for negative and positive phase shifts are not shown in Fig. 5.

  7. We note here briefly that Experiment 1 was preceded by a very similar experiment in which the timing perturbations were event onset shifts (Repp, 2002) rather than phase shifts. Event onset shifts change both IOIs in a cycle in a complementary fashion, thereby creating a larger change in interval ratio than a phase shift created in the present experiment. However, the results were almost identical and therefore are not reported here in detail. In the perception task, participants generally responded to the change in the shorter IOI, but the pattern of asymmetries was the same as in Fig. 4. The analysis of PCRs yielded only one reliable effect, a larger PCR in the 1:1 than in the 4:5 rhythm (cf. Fig. 5).

  8. It did not seem necessary to consider the results with author BHR’s data excluded because of his small PCRs in isochronous rhythms. His mean PCR was rather small in 360/360 but comparable to that of some other participants in 840/840.

References

  • Bharucha, J. J., & Pryor, J. H. (1986). Disrupting the isochrony underlying rhythm: An asymmetry in discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 137–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. American Journal of Psychology, 6, 145–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brochard, R., Abecasis, D., Potter, D., Ragot, R., & Drake, C. (2003). The “ticktock” of our internal clock: Direct brain evidence of subjective accents in isochronous sequences. Psychological Science, 14, 362–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S. (2007). Contagious heterophony: A new theory about the origins of music. Musicae Scientiae, 11, 3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collier, G. L., & Wright, C. E. (1995). Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 602–627.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, G., & Meyer, L. B. (1960). The rhythmic structure of music. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doumas, M., & Wing, A. M. (2007). Timing and trajectory in rhythm production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 442–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1946). Contribution à l’étude du rythme en tant que forme temporelle [Contribution to the study of rhythm as temporal form]. Journal de psychologie normale et pathologique, 39, 283–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friberg, A., & Sundström, A. (2002). Swing ratios and ensemble timing in jazz performance: Evidence for a common rhythmic pattern. Music Perception, 19, 333–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabrielsson, A., Bengtsson, I., & Gabrielsson, B. (1983). Performance of musical rhythm in 3/4 and 6/8 meter. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 24, 193–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hannon, E. E., & Trehub, S. E. (2005). Tuning into musical rhythms: Infants learn more readily than adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 12639–12643.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hary, D., & Moore, G. P. (1985). Temporal tracking and synchronization strategies. Human Neurobiology, 4, 73–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hary, D., & Moore, G. P. (1987). Synchronizing human movement with an external clock source. Biological Cybernetics, 56, 305–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC). The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Honing, H., & de Haas, W. B. (2008). Swing once more: Relating timing and tempo in expert jazz drumming. Music Perception, 25, 471–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. R., & McAuley, J. D. (2005). Time judgments in global temporal contexts. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 398–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drumming: Social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 299–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Large, E. W., Fink, P., & Kelso, J. A. S. (2002). Tracking simple and complex sequences. Psychological Research, 66, 3–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, J. (1997). Music in the moment. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • London, J. M. (2001). Rhythm. In Grove Music Online. Oxford Music Online, http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/45963.

  • London, J. M. (2004). Hearing in time. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mates, J., Radil, T., Müller, U., & Pöppel, E. (1994). Temporal integration in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 332–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merker, B. H., Madison, G. S., & Eckerdal, P. (2009). On the role and origin of isochrony in human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex, 45, 4–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parducci, A. (1974). Contextual effects: A range-frequency analysis. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception, Vol. II: Psychophysical judgment and measurement (pp. 127–141). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, M. (1989). The relationship between variability of intertap intervals and interval duration. Psychological Research, 51, 38–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Povel, D.-J. (1981). Internal representation of simple temporal patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pressing, J. (1999). Cognitive complexity and the structure of musical patterns. http://www.psych.unimelb.edu.au/staff/jp/cog-music.pdf.

  • Repp, B. H. (1998a). Variations on a theme by Chopin: Relations between perception and production of deviations from isochrony in music. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 791–811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (1998b). Obligatory “expectations” of expressive timing induced by perception of musical structure. Psychological Research, 61, 33–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2000). Compensation for subliminal timing perturbations in perceptual-motor synchronization. Psychological Research, 63, 106–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2001). Phase correction, phase resetting, and phase shifts after subliminal timing perturbations in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 600–621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2002). Automaticity and voluntary control of phase correction following event onset shifts in sensorimotor synchronization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 410–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2003). Rate limits in sensorimotor synchronization with auditory and visual sequences: The synchronization threshold and the benefits and costs of interval subdivision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, 355–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2005). Sensorimotor synchronization: A review of the tapping literature. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 969–992.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2006). Does an auditory perceptual illusion affect on-line auditory action control? The case of (de)accentuation and synchronization. Experimental Brain Research, 168, 493–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2008a). Multiple temporal references in sensorimotor synchronization with metrical auditory sequences. Psychological Research, 72, 79–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2008b). Perfect phase correction in synchronization with slow auditory sequences. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40, 363–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2009). Segregated in perception, integrated for action: Immunity of rhythmic sensorimotor coordination to auditory stream segregation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 426–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2010a). Sensorimotor synchronization and perception of timing: Effects of music training and task experience. Human Movement Science, 29, 200–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H. (2010b). Temporal evolution of the phase correction response in synchronization of taps with perturbed two-interval rhythms (Submitted).

  • Repp, B. H., London, J., & Keller, P. E. (2005). Production and synchronization of uneven rhythms at fast tempi. Music Perception, 23, 61–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H., London, J., & Keller, P. E. (2008). Phase correction in sensorimotor synchronization with nonisochronous sequences. Music Perception, 26, 171–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H., London, J., & Keller, P. E. (2010). Detuning of the 1:2 attractor ratio in production of two-interval rhythms. Manuscript in preparation.

  • Sadakata, M., Desain, P., & Honing, H. (2006). The Bayesian way to relate rhythm perception and production. Music Perception, 23, 269–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg, E. G. (2001). Asymmetries in the discrimination of musical intervals: Going out-of-tune is more noticeable than going in-tune. Music Perception, 19, 223–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Prediction of external events with our motor system: Towards a new framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 211–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semjen, A., & Ivry, R. B. (2001). The coupled oscillator model of between-hand coordination in alternate-hand tapping: A reappraisal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 251–265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shmulevich, I., & Povel, D.-J. (2000). Measures of temporal pattern complexity. Journal of New Music Research, 29, 61–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, J. S., Hannon, E. E., Large, E. W., & Christiansen, M. H. (2006). Synchronization and continuation tapping to complex meters. Music Perception, 24, 135–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S., Knoll, R. L., & Zukofsky, P. (1982). Timing by skilled musicians. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (pp. 181–239). Orlando, FL: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Summers, J. J., Bell, R., & Burns, B. D. (1989). Perceptual and motor factors in the imitation of simple temporal patterns. Psychological Research, 50, 23–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Summers, J. J., Hawkins, S. R., & Mayers, H. (1986). Imitation and production of interval ratios. Perception & Psychophysics, 39, 437–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thul, E., & Toussaint, G. T. (2008). On the relation between rhythm complexity measures and human rhythmic performance. In B. P. Desai (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2008 C 3 S 2 E conference (pp. 199–204). Montréal, Québec, May 12–13.

  • Tuller, B., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1989). Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects. Experimental Brain Research, 75, 306–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1978). On the temporal control of rhythmic performance. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance VII (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vorberg, D., & Hambuch, R. (1984). Timing of two-handed rhythmic performance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 390–406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vorberg, D., & Wing, A. (1996). Modeling variability and dependence in timing. In H. Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action (Vol. 2, pp. 181–262). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamanishi, J., Kawato, M., & Suzuki, R. (1980). Two coupled oscillators as a model for the coordinated finger tapping by both hands. Biological Cybernetics, 37, 219–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yee, W., Holleran, S., & Jones, M. R. (1994). Sensitivity to event timing in regular and irregular sequences: Influences of musical skill. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 461–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Science Foundation grant BCS-0642506.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bruno H. Repp.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Repp, B.H., London, J. & Keller, P.E. Perception–production relationships and phase correction in synchronization with two-interval rhythms. Psychological Research 75, 227–242 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0301-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0301-8

Keywords

Navigation