Skip to main content
Log in

In hindsight, life flows from left to right

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research PRPF Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Three experiments investigated the mental representation of meaningful event sequences. Experiment 1 used extended (5 min long) naturalistic scenes excerpted from commercial movies. Experiments 2 and 3 presented everyday activities by means of sequences of six photographs. All experiments found both left–right and distance effects in an order decision task, suggesting that when contemplated in hindsight, experienced events unfold along a left-to-right analogical mental line. Present results are discussed in the context of the mental representation of other kinds of ordinal sequences, and other left–right effects reported in non-ordinal domains.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The counterbalancing factor was not included in the design. Although there could be interactions with this factor, they would be of no theoretical value. Counterbalancing assures that order-practice effects are controlled for. Sometimes it is useful to include it as a factor in the analyses to take off some error variance and improve the power of the analysis, but it is not necessary in the present experiments, as all ANOVAs already provide significant results for the factors of interest.

References

  • Altmann, L. J. P., Saleem, A., Kendall, D., Heilman, K. M., & Rothi, L. J. G. (2006). Orthographic directionality and thematic role illustration in English and Arabic. Brain and Language, 97, 306–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, A. M., Kim, M., Crucian, G. P., & Heilman, K. M. (2002). Spatial bias: Effects of early reading direction on Korean subjects. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1003–1012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Kerzel, D. (2004). A Simon Effect With Stationary Moving Stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 39–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, L. K., Hess, U., & Wolford, G. (1993). Transformations for within-subject designs: A Monte-Carlo investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 566–579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where Language, Culture, and Cognition Meet (pp. 9–34). Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappelletti, M., Freeman, E. D., & Cipolotti, L. (2007). The middle house or the middle floor: Bisecting horizontal and vertical number lines in neglect. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2989–3000.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102, 101–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee, A. (2001). Language and space: Some interactions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 55–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee, A., Maher, L. M., Gonzalez-Rothi, L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Asyntactic thematic role assignment: The use of a temporal-spatial strategy. Brain and Language, 49, 125–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chatterjee, A., Southwood, M. H., & Basilico, D. (1999). Verbs, events and spatial representations. Neuropsychologia, 37, 395–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doricci, F., Guariglia, P., Gasparini, M., & Tomaiuolo, F. (2005). Dissociation between physical and mental number line bisection in right hemisphere brain damage. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1663–1665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W., & Fischer, M. H. (2005). Spatial Representation of Numbers. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 43–54). Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 555–556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2003). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized. Cognition, 87, B87–B95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gevers, W., Reynvoet, B., & Fias, W. (2004). The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organised: Evidence from days of the week. Cortex, 40, 171–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longo, M. R., & Lourenço, S. F. (2007). Spatial attention and the mental number line: Evidence for characteristic biases and compression. Neuropsychologia, 45, 1400–1407.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 16, 149–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maas, A., & Russo, A. (2003). Directional bias in the mental representation of spatial events: Nature or culture? Psychological Science, 14, 296–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maher, L. M., Chatterjee, A., Gonzalez-Rothi, L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Agrammatic sentence production: The use of a temporal-spatial strategy. Brain and Language, 49, 105–124.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgements of numerical inequality. Nature, 215, 1519–1520.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: convergent evidence from aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cogn Sci, 30, 1–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Funes, M. J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2008) Thinking about time makes attention move to the sides (submitted)

  • Price, M. C., & Mentzoni, R. A. (2008). Where is January? The month-SNARC effect in sequence-form synaesthetes. Cortex, 44, 890–907.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Priftis, K., Zorzi, M., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umiltà, C. (2006). Explicit vs. implicit processing of representational space in neglect: Dissociations in accessing the mental number line. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 680–688.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, R., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: a general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 416–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rinaldi, M. C., & Pizzamiglio, L. (2006). When space merges into language. Neuropsychologia, 44, 556–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoianov, I., Kramer, P., Umiltá, C., & Zorzi, M. (2008). Visuospatial priming of the mental number line. Cognition, 106, 770–779.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Funes, M. J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30, 745–757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (2004). Narratives of space, time, and life. Mind and Language, 19, 380–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 515–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A. & Shallice, T. (in press) An effect of spatial-temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time. Cognition.

  • Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 483–488.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weger, U. & Pratt, J. (in press) Time flies like an arrow: Shifting spatial attention in response to adverbs of time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

  • Zacks, J. M., & Tversky, B. (2001). Event structure in perception and conception. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 3–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zacks, J. M., Tversky, B., & Iyer, G. (2001). Perceiving, remembering, and communicating structure in events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 29–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the REVERSED SNARC effect in English, Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5, 165–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., & Umiltà, C. (2002). Neglect disrupts the mental number line. Nature, 417, 138–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zorzi, M., Priftis, K., Meneghello, F., Marenzi, R., & Umiltà, C. (2006). The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: Evidence from neglect. Neuropsychologia, 44, 1061–1067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The present research was supported by grant SEJ2006-04732 of the Spanish Ministry of Education and Science, Julio Santiago (PI). Portions of this research were presented at the Theme Session on Conceptual Projection, New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics Conference, Brighton, UK, October 23rd–25th, 2005, and also at the 10th International Cognitive Linguistics Conference, Krakow, Poland, July 15th–20th, 2007. Special thanks are due to Ana Luque for volunteering to be the character of the event sequences in Experiments 2 and 3.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Julio Santiago.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Santiago, J., Román, A., Ouellet, M. et al. In hindsight, life flows from left to right. Psychological Research 74, 59–70 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0220-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0220-0

Keywords

Navigation