Skip to main content
Log in

False memory following rapidly presented lists: the element of surprise

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article examines a false memory phenomenon, the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) effect, consisting of high false alarms for a prototype word (e.g., SLEEP) following a study list consisting of its associates (NIGHT, DREAM, etc.). This false recognition is thought to occur because prototypes, although not presented within a study list, are highly activated by their semantic association with words that are in the list. The authors present an alternative explanation of the effect, based on the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. According to that account, false (and true) familiarity results when a comparison between expectations and outcomes within a processing episode causes surprise. Experiment 1 replicates the DRM effect. Experiment 2 shows that a similar effect can occur when participants are shown lists of unrelated words and are then surprised by a recognition target. Experiments 3 and 4 show that the DRM effect itself is abolished when participants are prevented from being surprised by prototypes presented as recognition targets. It is proposed that the DRM effect is best understood through the principles of construction, evaluation, and attribution.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Not all surprises cause this misattribution, even when regarded as funny. For example, Whittlesea and Williams (2001b) presented test words as the last word of familiar phrases. They compared claims of recognition for words presented as clang associations (e.g., Row, row row your GOAT) with words presented in their original context (e.g., Row, row row your BOAT) or words simply mismatching the context (e.g., Row, row row your SHEEP). The participants laughed at many of the clangs, but that manipulation had no observable effect on claims of remembering. The difference between the two cases of surprise appears to be that when dealing with items like PHRAUG, people interpret their subsequent processing as matching well, although surprisingly so, with earlier processing, whereas presentations such as GOAT seem to be surprisingly wrong.

  2. The rate of false alarms for prototypes might be expected to be equal in this case to those for nonprototoype items, but it is actually less. We suspect that the difference occurred because the prototypes actually did summarize the lists satisfactorily, whereas the nonprototoype words the participant generated were much less representative. Generating a prototype would thus give the participant a sense of having produced a word totally under control of the attempt to summarize, and so would be attributed only to that source. In contrast, because generated nonprototype words were less representative of the list, they would not so clearly appear to be a product of a summarization process, so that the participants could not as definitely attribute their occurrence in mind to that source.

References

  • Arndt, J., & Hirshman, E. (1998). True and false recognition in MINERVA2: Explanations from a global-matching perspective. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 371–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, S., Moscovitch, M., Behrmann, M., & Joordens, S. (1997). Long-term semantic priming: A computational account and empirical evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 1059–1082.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J., Wright, R., Reyna, V. F., & Mojardin, A. H. (2001). Conjoint recognition and phantom recollection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 307–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bredart, S. (2000) When false memories do not occur: Not thinking of the lure or remembering that it was not heard? Memory, 8, 123–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deese, J. (1959). On the predictions of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallo, D. A., Roberts, M. J., & Seamon, J. G. (1997). Remembering words not presented in lists: Can we avoid creating false memories? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 271–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallo, D. A., Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (2001). Associative false recognition occurs without strategic criterion shifts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 579–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, T. W., Hicks, J. L., Marsh, R. L., & Ritschel, L. (2003). Measuring activation level of critical lures in the Deese–Roediger–McDermott paradigm. American Journal of Psychology, 116, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks, J. L., & Marsh, R. L. (1999). Attempts to reduce the incidence of false recall with source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1195–1209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, A. D., & Whittlesea, B. W. A. (2003). Long-term semantic transfer: An overlapping-operations account. Memory & Cognition, 31, 401–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 3–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joordens, S., & Becker, S. (1997). The long and short of semantic priming effects in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 1083–1105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joordens, S., & Besner, D. (1992). Priming effects that span an intervening unrelated word: Implications for models of memory representation and retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 483–491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, M., Henkel, L., & Johnson, M. K. (1997). Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire compared. Memory & Cognition, 25, 826–837.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, K. B. (1996). The persistence of false memories in list recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 212–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, K. B. (1997). Priming on perceptual implicit memory tests can be achieved through presentation of associates. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 582–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. III (1998). False recognition of associates can be resistant to an explicit warning to subjects and an immediate recognition probe. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 508–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, K. B., & Watson, J. M. (2001). The rise and fall of false recall: The impact of presentation duration. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 160–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy, C. L., Nelson, D. L., & Komatsu, T. (1999). What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of interitem associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1177–1194.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKone, E., & Murphy, B. (2000). Implicit false memory: Effects of modality and multiple study presentations on long-lived semantic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 89–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T. P. (1992). Theories of priming. I. Associative distance and lag. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 1173–1190.

    Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K., de Sa, V., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1997). On the nature and scope of featural representations in word meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 99–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, M. B., & Wolford, G. L. (1999). Theoretical commentary: The role of criterion shift in false memory. Psychological Review, 106, 398–405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H., & Keefe, D. E. (1989). Semantic context effects on visual word processing: A hybrid prospective/retrospective processing theory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol 24 (pp. 207–248). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., Schreiber, T. A., & McEvoy, C. L (1992). Processing implicit and explicit representations. Psychological Review, 99, 322–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McKinney, V. M., Gee, N. R., & Janczura, G. A. (1998). Interpreting the influence of implicitly activated memories on recall and recognition. Psychological Review, 105, 299–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, K. A., & Schacter, D. L. (1997). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories. Memory & Cognition, 25, 838–848.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. C., & Booth, J. R. (2000). Individual and developmental differences in semantic priming: Empirical and computational support for a single-mechanism account of lexical processing. Psychological Review, 107, 786–823.

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, J. D. (1996). From a passing thought to a false memory in 2 minutes: Confusing real and illusory events. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 3, 105–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, K., & Roediger, H. L. III (1997). Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychological Science, 8, 231–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (2000). Tricks of memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 123–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. III, Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2000). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3, 385–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. III, Balota, D. A., & Watson, J. M. (2001). Spreading activation and arousal of false memories. In H. L. Roediger III, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honour of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 95–115). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., & Pradre, D. (1996). The neuropsychology of memory illusions: False recall and recognition in amnesic patients. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 319–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seamon, J. G., Luo, C. R., & Gallo, D. A. (1998). Creating false memories of words with or without recognition of list items: Evidence for nonconscious processes. Psychological Science, 9, 20–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seamon, J. G., Luo, C. R., Schwartz, M. A., Jones, K. J., Lee, D. M., & Jones, S. J. (2002). Repetition can have similar or different effects on accurate and false recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 323–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stadler, M. A., Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (1999). Norms for words that create false memories. Memory & Cognition, 27, 494–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A. (2002a). False memory and the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: The prototype-familiarity illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 96–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A. (2002b). Two routes to remembering (and another to remembering not). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 325–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Jacoby, L. L. (1990). Interaction of prime repetition with visual degradation: Is priming a retrieval phenomenon? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 546–565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Leboe, J. P. (2003). Two fluency heuristics (and how to tell them apart). Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 62–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (1998). Why do strangers feel familiar, but friends don’t? The unexpected basis of feelings of familiarity. Acta Psychologica, 96, 141–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2000). The source of feelings of familiarity: The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 547–565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001a). The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. I. The heuristic basis of feelings of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001b). The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. II. Expectation, uncertainty, surprise and feelings of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 14–33.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to B.W.A.W. and M.E.J.M.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bruce W. A. Whittlesea.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Whittlesea, B.W.A., Masson, M.E.J. & Hughes, A.D. False memory following rapidly presented lists: the element of surprise. Psychological Research 69, 420–430 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0213-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0213-1

Keywords

Navigation