Skip to main content
Log in

Tau and Kappa effects in physical space: the case of audition

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The organization of spatio-temporal information in an auditory memory task was studied in two experiments. Stimuli consisted of four different configurations of eight sequentially presented beeps. In two configurations, the stimuli were space–time congruent, with (constant or variable) inter-stimulus distances corresponding to (constant or variable) inter-stimulus time intervals. In the other two configurations, the stimuli were space–time incongruent, with (constant or variable) inter-stimulus distances not corresponding to (variable or constant) inter-stimulus time intervals. After a learning phase consisting of 20 presentations of the target configuration, participants reproduced the spatial (Experiment 1) or temporal (Experiment 2) characteristics of the target 60 times in succession without re-examining the target configuration. Accuracy (with respect to the target) and variability (between responses) were found to evolve independently. In the incongruent space–time conditions, effects of variable inter-stimulus time intervals or distances on the reproduction of, respectively, constant distances (Tau effect) or constant time intervals (Kappa effect) were observed, while the reverse was not the case. Thus, dimensional interference occurred when the dimension to be ignored was variable. The results are discussed in the light of the distinction between properties of the stabilized mental image and the process of stabilization.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The conditions are labeled in the same way that the first experiment. The first letter corresponds to the spatial dimension and the second letter to the temporal dimension

References

  • Anderson, N.H. (1974). Algebraic models in perception. In E.C. Carterette & M.P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception (Vol. 2). New York: Academic Press.

  • Benussi, V. (1913). Versuche zur Analyse taktil erweckter Scheinbewegungen. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 36, 58–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bill, J.C., & Teft, L.W. (1969). Space–time relations: Effects of time on perceived visual extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 196–199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bill, J.C., & Teft, L.W. (1972). Space–time relations: The effects of variations in stimulus and interstimulus interval duration on perceived visual extent. Acta Psychologica, 36, 358–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bremer, C.D., Pittenger, J.B., Warren, R., & Jenkins, J.J. (1977). An illusion of auditory saltation similar to the cutaneous “rabbit”. American Journal of Psychology, 90, 645–654.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, W.H. (1903). Retroactive amnesia: Illustrative cases and a tentative explanation. American Journal of Psychology, 14, 382–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chatfield, C. (1975). The analysis of time series: Theories and practice. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Y., Repp, B.H., & Patel, A.D. (2002). Spectral decomposition of variability in synchronization and continuation tapping: Comparisons between auditory and visual pacing and feedback conditions. Human Movement Science, 21, 515–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Hansel, C.E.M., & Sylvester, J.D. (1953). A new phenomenon in time judgement. Nature, London, 172, 901.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collyer, C.E. (1977). Discrimination of spatial and temporal intervals defined by three light flashes: Effects of spacing on temporal judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 357–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 164–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P.M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 381–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geldard, F.A., & Sherrick, C.E. (1972). The cutaneous “rabbit”: A perceptual illusion. Science, 178, 178–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilden, D. L., Thornton, T., & Mallon, M. W. (1995). 1/f noise in human cognition. Science, 267, 1837–1839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilden, D.L. (2001). Cognitive emissions of 1/f noise. Psychological Review, 108, 33–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giraudo, M.D., & Pailhous, J. (1999). Dynamic instability of visuo-spatial images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 6, 1495–1516.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A.,& Jona, M. (1991). Temporal coding in rhythm tasks revealed by modality effects. Memory and Cognition, 19, 514–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R.G. (1987). Environmental cognition. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of Environmental Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 131–174). New-York: Wiley.

  • Grondin, S., & Rousseau, R. (1991). Judging the relative duration of multimodal short empty time intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 245–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H. (1930). The Tau effect: An example of psychological relativity. Science, 71, 536–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., & King, S.M. (1931). The Tau effect: An example of psychological relativity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 202–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B., & Huang, Y.L. (1982). Space–time dependencies in psychological judgment of extent and duration: algebraic model of the Tau and Kappa effects. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 128–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J.A.S. (1981). On the oscillatory basis of movement. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D. (2000). Learning of spatial and temporal patterns in sequential hand movements. Cognitive Brain Research, 9, 35–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monno, A., Chardenon, A., Temprado, J.J., Zanone, P.G., & Laurent, M. (2000). Effects of attention on phase transitions between bimanual coordination patterns: a behavioral and cost analysis in humans. Neuroscience Letters. 283: 93–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, K.M., & Corcos, D.M. (1993). Issues in variability and motor control. In K.M. Newell & D.M. Corcos (Eds.), Variability and motor control (pp. 1–12). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, C.V., & Lee, S.G. (1972). The effect of real and imputed distance on judgments of time: Some experiments on the Kappa effect. Psychonomic Science, 29, 207–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pailhous, J., & Bonnard, M. (1992). Steady state variability of human gait: Role of optical flow. Behavior Brain Research, 47, 181–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. (1980). The development of attentional mechanisms. In P.M.A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and Performance (Vol. V). New York: Academic Press.

  • Price-Williams, D.R. (1954). The Kappa effect. Nature, 173, 363–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B.H., & Penel, A. (2002). Auditory dominance in temporal processing: New evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1085–1099.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B.H., & Penel, A. (2004). Rhythmic movement is attracted more strongly to auditory than to visual rhythms. Psychological Research, 68(4), 252–270

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarrazin, J.C., Giraudo, M.D.,& Pailhous, J. (2001). Dynamics space and time dimensions in the memorizing process. In G. Burton and R. Schmidt (Eds.). Studies in perception and action VI (pp. 209–212). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarrazin, J.C., Giraudo, M.D. Pailhous, J., & Bootsma, R.J. (2004). Tau and Kappa effects revisited: Dynamics of balancing space and time in Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30,3, 411–430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R.A., (1988). Motor control and learning. A behavioral emphasis. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherrick, C. E. (1974). Sensory processes. In L. L. Elliott& J. A. Swets (Eds.), Psychology and the handicapped child (pp.13–39). Washington: U. S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sokolov, E.N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen, T.A., & Bardy, B.G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 24, 195–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, S., & Cavanagh, P. (1997). Focused attention distorts visual space: An attentional repulsion effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 443–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temprado, J.J., Chardenon, A., & Laurent, M. (2001). Interplay of biomechanical and neuromuscular constraints on pattern stability and attentional demands in a bimanual coordination task. Neuroscience Letters, 303, 127–131.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wade, N.J., & Heller, D. (1997). Scopes of perception: the experimental manipulation of space and time. Psychological Research, 60(4), 227–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D.B., Greenberg, A.R., & Thomas, R.C. (1997). Response-to-stimulus interval does not affect implicit motor sequence learning, but does affect performance. Memory and Cognition, 25, 534–542.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Direction des Forces et de la Prospective de la Direction Générale de l’Armement (DGA) and by the Marie Wilson Howells Bequest to the Department of Psychology, University of Arkansas at Little Rock. This research was conducted while Jean-Christophe Sarrazin was at University of Arkansas.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marie-Dominique Giraudo.

Appendices

Appendix 1

To replicate an invisible motion in the horizontal plane, software developed at the Movement and Perception Laboratory (Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique) uses the Inverse Distance Clamped Model, extended to guarantee that for distances below REFERENCE_DISTANCE, gain is clamped. This mode is equivalent to the IASIG I3DL2 (and DS3D) distance model.

$$\begin{aligned} {\text{dist}} =& {\text{max}}({\text{dist}},{\text{REFERENCE}}\_{\text{DISTANCE}}); \\ {\text{dist}} =& {\text{min}}({\text{dist}},{\text{MAX}}\_{\text{DISTANCE}}); \\ {\text{G}}\_{\text{dB}} =& {\text{GAIN}} - 20*\log 10(1 + {\text{ROLLOFF}}\_{\text{FACTOR}}*({\text{dist}} - {\text{REFERENCE}}\_{\text{DISTANCE}}) / {\text{REFERENCE}}\_{\text{DISTANCE}}); \\ {\text{G}}\_{\text{dB}} =& {\text{min}}({\text{G}}\_{\text{dB}},{\text{MAX}}\_{\text{GAIN}}); \\ {\text{G}}\_{\text{dB}} =& {\text{max}}({\text{G}}\_{\text{dB}},{\text{MIN}}\_{\text{GAIN}});\\ \end{aligned}$$

The Inverse Distance Clamped Model is an extension of the well-known OpenAL sound system.

OpenAL website: http://www.openal.org

Appendix 2

The correction of the gap between the first four and the last four dots was performed for each reproduced configuration. The positions of the dots are denoted p 1, p 2, ..., p 8, while the distances between adjacent pairs of dots are denoted d 1, d 2, ... d 7. Two standard deviations were computed. The first, SD1, was computed over all distances. The second, SD2, was computed over d 1, d 2, d 3, d 5, d 6, and d 7, i.e., with the size of the central gap removed from the calculation. The correction of the gap was accomplished by repositioning the dots so as to make SD2 approximately equal to SD1. To do so, we first computed M1, the mean of d 1, d 2, and d 3, and M2, the mean of d 5, d 6, and d 7. The new positions of the dots were computed as follows: p 1=abs (p 1−0.25 M1); p 2=abs (p 2−0.125 M1); p 3=p 3; p 4=abs (p 4+0.125 M1); p 5=abs (p 5−0.125 M2); p 6=p 6; p 7=abs (p 7+0.125 M2); and p 8=abs (p 8+0.25 M2).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sarrazin, JC., Giraudo, MD. & Pittenger, J.B. Tau and Kappa effects in physical space: the case of audition. Psychological Research 71, 201–218 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0019-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-005-0019-1

Keywords

Navigation