Skip to main content
Log in

Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) recognize group membership via olfactory cues alone

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The ability to distinguish group members from conspecifics living in other groups is crucial for gregarious species. Olfaction is known to play a major role in group recognition and territorial defense in a wide range of mammalian taxa. Although primates have been typically regarded as microsmatic (having a poor sense of smell), increasing evidence suggests that olfaction may play a greater role in primates’ social life than previously assumed. In this study, we carried out behavioral bioassays using a signaler-receiver paradigm to investigate whether rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) can discriminate between body odors of female group members and females from different social groups. We conducted the study on the research island Cayo Santiago, Puerto Rico, in the non-mating season and controlled for kinship and familiarity using extensive pedigree and demographic data. Our results indicate that both males and females inspect out-group odors significantly longer than in-group odors. Males licked odors more often than females, and older animals licked more often than younger ones. Furthermore, individuals tended to place their nose longer towards odors when the odor donor’s group rank was higher than the rank of their own group. Reuse of odor samples decreased odor intensity (rated by human experimenters) during the course of a given test day and with longer exposure to ambient air; however, the reuse of odor samples did not significantly influence the response behaviors. Our findings uncover key roles of olfactory communication in a species not possessing distinct scent glands and thus shed light into the evolution of primate olfactory communication.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Altmann J (1990) Primate males go where the females are. Anim Behav 39:193–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Archie EA, Theis KR (2011) Animal behaviour meets microbial ecology. Anim Behav 82:425–436

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen H (2008) Analyzing linguistic data: a practical introduction to statistics using R, 1st edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baron G, Frahm H, Bhatnagar KP, Stephan H (1983) Comparison of brain structure volumes in insectivora and primates. III. Main olfactory bulb (MOB). J Hirnforsch 24:551–568

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J Mem Lang 68:255–278

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B (2015) lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-8. Available from: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4

  • Bentson KL, Capitanio JP, Mendoza SP (2003) Cortisol responses to immobilization with Telazol or ketamine in baboons (Papio cynocephalus/anubis) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Med Primatol 32:148–160

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berard JD, Nürnberg P, Epplen JT, Schmidtke J (1993) Male rank, reproductive behavior, and reproductive success in free-ranging rhesus macaques. Primates 34:481–489

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bercovitch FB (1991) Mate selection, consortship formation, and reproductive tactics in adult female savanna baboons. Primates 32:437–452

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berghänel A (2009) Die sozioökologische Basis der sozialen Beziehungen zwischen adulten Berberaffenmännchen und deren physiologische Stresskorrelate. Diploma thesis, Universität Jena

  • Bergman TJ, Kitchen DM (2009) Comparing responses to novel objects in wild baboons (Papio ursinus) and geladas (Theropithecus gelada). Anim Cogn 12:63–73

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergman TJ, Sheehan MJ (2013) Social knowledge and signals in primates. Am J Primatol 75:683–694

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Whitten PL (2006) Interactions in male baboons: the importance of both males’ testosterone. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 59:480–489

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloss J, Acree TE, Bloss JM, Hood WR, Kunz TH (2002) Potential use of chemical cues for colony-mate recognition in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J Chem Ecol 28:819–834

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boelkins RC, Wilson AP (1972) Intergroup social dynamics of the Cayo Santiago rhesus (Macaca mulatta) with special reference to changes in group membership by males. Primates 13:125–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch C, Crockford C, Herbinger I, Wittig R, Moebius Y, Normand E (2008) Intergroup conflicts among chimpanzees in Taï National Park: lethal violence and the female perspective. Am J Primatol 70:519–532

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, White JSS (2009) Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 24:127–135

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonadonna F, Hesters F, Jouventin P (2003) Scent of a nest: discrimination of own-nest odours in Antarctic prions, Pachyptila desolata. Behav Biol Sociobiol 54:174–178

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonavita-Cougourdan A, Clément JL, Lange C (1987) Nestmate recognition: the role of cuticular hydrocarbons in the ant Camponotus vagus Scop. J Entomol Sci 22:1–10

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Breed MD (1983) Nestmate recognition in honey bees. Anim Behav 31:86–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brereton AR (1981) Inter-group consorting by a free-ranging female rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Primates 22:417–423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruschini C, Cervo R, Cini A, Pieraccini G, Pontieri L, Signorotti L, Turilazzi S (2011) Cuticular hydrocarbons rather than peptides are responsible for nestmate recognition in Polistes dominulus. Chem Senses 36:715–723

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buesching CD, Waterhouse JS, Macdonald DW (2002) Gas-chromatographic analyses of the subcaudal gland secretion of the European badger (Meles meles). Part I: chemical differences related to individual parameters. J Chem Ecol 28:41–56

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buhl JS, Aure B, Ruiz-Lambides A, Gonzales-Martinez J, Platt ML, Brent LJ (2012) Response of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) to the body of a group member that died from a fatal attack. Int J Primatol 33:860–871

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter CR (1942) Sexual behavior of free ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). I. Specimens procedures and behavioral characteristics of estrus. II. Periodicity of estrus, homosexual, autoerotic and nonconformist behavior. J Comp Psychol 32:113–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castles DL, Whiten A, Aureli F (1999) Social anxiety, relationships and self-directed behaviour among wild female olive baboons. Anim Behav 58:1207–1215

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charpentier MJE, Boulet M, Drea CM (2008) Smelling right: the scent of male lemurs advertises genetic quality and relatedness. Mol Ecol 17:3225–3233

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charpentier MJE, Crawford JC, Boulet M, Drea CM (2010) Message “scent”: lemurs detect the genetic relatedness and quality of conspecifics via olfactory cues. Anim Behav 80:101–108

  • Charpentier MJE, Mboumba S, Ditsoga C, Drea CM (2013) Nasopalatine ducts and flehmen behavior in the mandrill: reevaluating olfactory communication in Old World primates. Am J Primatol 75:703–714

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke PMR, Barrett L, Henzi SP (2009) What role do olfactory cues play in chacma baboon mating? Am J Primatol 71:493–502

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colvin J (1983) Influences of the social situation on male emigration. In: Hinde RA (ed) Primate social relationships. An integrated approach. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 160–171

    Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2015) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.r-project.org/

  • Coulon M, Baudoin C, Heyman Y, Deputte BL (2010) Cattle discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics by using only head visual cues. Anim Cogn 14:279–290

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dobson AJ (2002) An introduction to generalized linear models, 2nd edn. Chapman & Hall/CRC, Boca Raton

    Google Scholar 

  • Donati G, Santini L, Razafindramanana J et al (2013) (Un-)expected nocturnal activity in “diurnal” Lemur catta supports cathemerality as one of the key adaptations of the lemurid radiation. Am J Phys Anthropol 150:99–106

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drea CM (2015) D’scent of man: a comparative survey of primate chemosignaling in relation to sex. Horm Behav 68:117–133

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drea CM, Boulet M, Delbarco-Trillo J, Greene LK, Sacha CR, Goodwin TE, Dubay GR (2013) The “secret” in secretions: methodological considerations in deciphering primate olfactory communication. Am J Primatol 75:621–642

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drickamer LC (1974) A ten-year summary of reproductive data for free-ranging Macaca mulatta. Folia Primatol 21:61–80

  • Dubuc C, Muniz L, Heistermann M, Widdig A, Engelhardt A (2012) Do males time their mate-guarding effort with the fertile phase in order to secure fertilisation in Cayo Santiago rhesus macaques? Horm Behav 61:696–705

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar I, Carmichael M (1981) The response of male dogs to urine from other males. Behav Neural Biol 31:465–470

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field A (2009) Discovering statistics using SPSS, 3 Pap/Cdr. Sage Publications Ltd., Los Angeles

  • Flack JC, de Waal FBM (2004) Dominance style, social power, and conflict management in macaque societies: a conceptual framework. In: Thierry B, Singh M, Kaumanns W (eds) Macaque societies: a model for the study of social organization. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 157–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Fornasieri I, Roeder JJ (1992) Behavioral responses to own and other species’ scent marks in Lemur fulvus and Lemur macaco. J Chem Ecol 18:2069–2082

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forstmeier W, Schielzeth H (2011) Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65:47–55

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox J, Weisberg S (2011) An {R} companion to applied regression, 2nd edn. Sage, Thousand Oaks

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuller GB, Hobson WC, Reyes FI, Winter JS, Faiman C (1984) Influence of restraint and ketamine anesthesia on adrenal steroids, progesterone, and gonadotropins in rhesus monkeys. P Soc Exp Biol Med 175:487–490

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann T (1987) A sternal gland in the siamang gibbon (Hylobates syndactylus). Int J Primatol 8:1–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gouzoules S, Gouzoules H (1987) Kinship. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 299–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Gustison M, Maclarnon A, Wiper S, Semple S (2012) An experimental study of behavioural coping strategies in free-ranging female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Stress 15:608–617

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris MA, Murie JO (1982) Responses to oral gland scents from different males in Columbian ground squirrels. Anim Behav 30:140–148

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hausfater G (1972) Intergroup behavior of free-ranging rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Folia Primatol 18:78–107

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauty GT, Yellin AM (1970) Activity cycles of the monkey (Macaca mulatta) under different environmental conditions. J Interdiscipl Cycle 1:181–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henzi SP, Lucas JW (1980) Observations on the inter-troop movement of adult vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Folia Primatol 33:220–235

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hepper PG, Wells DL (2010) Individually identifiable body odors are produced by the gorilla and discriminated by humans. Chem Senses 35:263–268

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hepper PG, Wells DL (2012) Olfactory discrimination in the western lowland gorilla, Gorilla gorilla gorilla. Primates 53:121–126

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herbinger I, Papworth S, Boesch C, Zuberbühler K (2009) Vocal, gestural and locomotor responses of wild chimpanzees to familiar and unfamiliar intruders: a playback study. Anim Behav 78:1389–1396

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heymann EW (2006) The neglected sense-olfaction in primate behavior, ecology, and evolution. Am J Primatol 68:519–524

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman C, Ruiz-Lambides AV, Davila E, Maldonado E, Gerald M, Dario Maestripieri D (2008) Sex differences in survival costs of reproduction in a promiscuous primate. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1711–1718

  • Joubert A, Vauclair J (1986) Reaction to novel objects in a troop of Guinea baboons: approach and manipulation. Behaviour 96:92–104

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann JH (1967) Social relations of adult males in a free-ranging band of rhesus monkeys. In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. University Of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 73–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawai M, Iwamoto T (1979) Nomadism and activities. In: Kawai M (ed) Ecological and sociological studies of gelada baboons. Karger, Basel, pp 251–278

    Google Scholar 

  • Keverne EB (1999) The vomeronasal organ. Science 286:716–720

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kitchen DM, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2005) Male chacma baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus) discriminate loud call contests between rivals of different relative ranks. Anim Cogn 8:1–6

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koford CB (1963) Group relations in an island colony of rhesus monkeys. In: Southwick CH (ed) Primate social behavior. Van Nostrand, New York, pp 136–152

    Google Scholar 

  • Krief S, Cibot M, Bortolamiol S, Seguya A, Krief JM, Masi S (2014) Wild chimpanzees on the edge: nocturnal activities in croplands. PLoS One 9, e109925

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kulahci IG, Drea CM, Rubenstein DI, Ghazanfar AA (2014) Individual recognition through olfactory-auditory matching in lemurs. Proc R Soc B 281:20140071

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laska M, Freyer D (1997) Olfactory discrimination ability for aliphatic esters in squirrel monkeys and humans. Chem Senses 22:457–465

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leclaire S, Nielsen JF, Drea CM (2014) Bacterial communities in meerkat anal scent secretions vary with host sex, age, and group membership. Behav Ecol 25:996–1004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemasson A, Boutin A, Boivin S, Blois-Heulin C, Hausberger M (2009) Horse (Equus caballus) whinnies: a source of social information. Anim Cogn 12:693–704

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenochova P, Roberts SC, Havlicek J (2008) Methods of human body odor sampling: the effect of freezing. Chem Senses 34:127–138

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lenoir A, Fresneau D, Errard C, Hefetz A (1999) Individuality and colonial identity in ants: the emergence of the social representation concept. In: Detrain C, Deneubourg JL, Pasteels JM (eds) Information processing in social insects. Springer, Basel, pp 219–237

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lindburg DG (1969) Rhesus monkeys: mating season mobility of adult males. Science 166:1176–1178

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loy J (1970) Behavioral responses of free-ranging rhesus monkeys to food shortage. Am J Phys Anthropol 33:263–271

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald EA, Fernandez-duque E, Evans S, Hagey LR (2008) Sex, age, and family differences in the chemical composition of owl monkey (Aotus nancymaae) subcaudal scent secretions. Am J Primatol 70:12–18

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maestripieri D (1993) Maternal anxiety in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Ethology 95:32–42

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maestripieri D, Schino G, Aureli F, Troisi A (1992) A modest proposal: displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Anim Behav 44:967–979

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manson JH (1995) Do female rhesus macaques choose novel males? Am J Primatol 37:285–296

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marriott BM, Roemer J, Sultana C (1989) An overview of the food intake patterns of the Cayo Santiago rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): report of a pilot study. P R Health Sci J 8:87–94

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin PC, Bateson P (1993) Measuring behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McComb K, Moss C, Sayialel S, Baker L (2000) Unusually extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants. Anim Behav 59:1103–1109

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melnick DJ, Pearl MC, Richard AF (1984) Male migration and inbreeding avoidance in wild rhesus monkeys. Am J Primatol 7:229–43

  • Mitani JC, Watts DP (2005) Correlates of territorial boundary patrol behaviour in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 70:1079–1086

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller-Schwarze D (1971) Pheromones in black-tailed deer (Odocoileus hemionus columbianus). Anim Behav 19:141–152

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Munakata Y, Santos LR, Spelke ES, Hauser MD, O’Reilly RC (2001) Visual representation in the wild: how rhesus monkeys parse objects. J Cognitive Neurosci 13:44–58

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nishikawa M, Mochida K (2010) Coprophagy-related interspecific nocturnal interactions between Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) and sika deer (Cervus nippon yakushimae). Primates 51:95–99

  • Nunn CL (1999) The number of males in primate social groups: a comparative test of the socioecological model. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 46:1–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palagi E, Dapporto L (2006) Beyond odor discrimination: demonstrating individual recognition by scent in Lemur catta. Chem Senses 31:437–443

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palombit RA (1994) Extra-pair copulations in a monogamous ape. Anim Behav 47:721–723

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Péron F, Ward R, Burman O (2014) Horses (Equus caballus) discriminate body odour cues from conspecifics. Anim Cogn 17:1007–1011

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfefferle D, Kazem AJN, Brockhausen RR, Ruiz-Lambides AV, Widdig A (2014a) Monkeys spontaneously discriminate their unfamiliar paternal kin under natural conditions using facial cues. Curr Biol 24:1806–1810

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfefferle D, Ruiz-Lambides AV, Widdig A (2014b) Female rhesus macaques discriminate unfamiliar paternal sisters in playback experiments—support for acoustic phenotype matching. Proc R Soc Lond B 281:20131628

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pokorny JJ, de Waal FBM (2009) Face recognition in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 123:151–160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Puri CP, Puri V, Anand Kumar TC (1981) Serum levels of testosterone, cortisol, prolactin and bioactive luteinizing hormone in adult male rhesus monkeys following cage-restraint or anaesthetizing with ketamine hydrochloride. Acta Endocrinol 97:118–124

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quick NJ, Janik VM (2012) Bottlenose dolphins exchange signature whistles when meeting at sea. Proc R Soc Lond B 279:2539–2545

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasa OAE (1973) Marking behaviour and its social significance in the African dwarf mongoose, Helogale undulata rufula. Z Tierpsychol 32:293–318

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rawlins RG, Kessler MJ (eds) (1986) The Cayo Santiago macaques. History, behavior and biology, Illustratedth edn. State University of New York Press, Albany

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts SC, Gosling LM, Carter V, Petrie M (2008) MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives. Proc R Soc B 275:2715–2722

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schell A, Rieck K, Schell K, Hammerschmidt K, Fischer J (2011) Adult but not juvenile Barbary macaques spontaneously recognize group members from pictures. Anim Cogn 14:503–509

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schielzeth H (2010) Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Method Ecol Evol 1:103–113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schielzeth H, Forstmeier W (2009) Conclusions beyond support: overconfident estimates in mixed models. Behav Ecol 20:416–420

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scordato ES, Drea CM (2007) Scents and sensibility: information content of olfactory signals in the ringtailed lemur, Lemur catta. Anim Behav 73:301–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scordato ES, Dubay G, Drea CM (2007) Chemical composition of scent marks in the ringtailed lemur (Lemur catta): glandular differences, seasonal variation, and individual signatures. Chem Senses 32:493–504

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Setchell JM, Vaglio S, Moggi-Cecchi J, Boscaro F, Calamai L, Knapp LA (2010) Chemical composition of scent-gland secretions in an Old World monkey (Mandrillus sphinx): influence of sex, male status, and individual identity. Chem Senses 35:205–220

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seth PK, Seth S (1986) Ecology and behavior of rhesus monkeys in India. In: Else JG, Lee PC (eds) Primate ecology and conservation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 192–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB (1999) Male bonnet macaques use information about third-party rank relationships to recruit allies. Anim Behav 58:45–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith TD, Bhatnagar KP (2004) Microsmatic primates: reconsidering how and when size matters. Anat Rec B New Anat 279:24–31

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith TE, Abbott DH, Tomlinson AJ, Mlotkiewicz JA (1997) Differential display of investigative behavior permits discrimination of scent signatures from familiar and unfamiliar socially dominant female marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). J Chem Ecol 23:2523–2546

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Southwick CH, Yongzu Z, Haisheng J, Zhenhe L, Wenyuan QU (1996) Population ecology of rhesus macaques in tropical and temperate habitats in China. In: Fa JE, Lindburg DG (eds) Evolution and ecology of macaque societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 95–105

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephan H, Bauchot R, Andy OJ (1970) Data on size of the brain and of various brain parts in insectivores and primates. In: Noback C, Montagna W (eds) The primate brain. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, pp 289–297

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan CL, Yang Y, Niu K (2013) Into the night: camera traps reveal nocturnal activity in a presumptive diurnal primate, Rhinopithecus brelichi. Primates 54:1–6

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tang Halpin Z (1976) The role of individual recognition by odors in the social interactions of the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). Behaviour 58:117–129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang Halpin Z (1986) Individual odors among mammals: origins and functions. Adv Stud Behav 16:39–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theis KR, Schmidt TM, Holekamp KE (2012) Evidence for a bacterial mechanism for group-specific social odors among hyenas. Sci Rep 2:615

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thomsen R, Soltis J (2004) Male masturbation in free-ranging Japanese macaques. Int J Primatol 25:1033–1041

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ueno Y (1994) Responses to urine odor in the tufted capuchin (Cebus apella). J Ethol 12:81–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Noordwijk MA (1985) Sexual behaviour of Sumatran long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Z Tierpsychol 70:277–296

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vessey SH (1973) Night observations of free‐ranging rhesus monkeys. Am J Phys Anthropol 38:613–619

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watts DP, Mitani JC (2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138:299–327

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickings EJ, Nieschlag E (1980) Pituitary response to LRH and TRH stimulation and peripheral steroid hormones in conscious and anaesthetized adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Eur J Endocrinol 93:287–293

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Widdig A (2002) Paternal kinship among adult female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). PhD thesis, Humboldt University Berlin

  • Widdig A, Nürnberg P, Krawczak M, Streich WJ, Bercovitch FB (2001) Paternal relatedness and age proximity regulate social relationships among adult female rhesus macaques. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98:13769–13773

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Widdig A, Bercovitch FB, Streich WJ, Sauermann U, Nürnberg P, Krawczak M (2004) A longitudinal analysis of reproductive skew in male rhesus macaques. Proc R Soc Lond B 271:819–826

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Widdig A, Nürnberg P, Bercovitch FB, Trefilov A, Berard JB, Kessler MJ, Schmidtke J, Streich WJ, Krawczak M (2006a) Consequences of group fission for the patterns of relatedness among rhesus macaques. Mol Ecol 15:3825–3832

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Widdig A, Streich WJ, Nürnberg P, Croucher PJP, Bercovitch FB, Krawczak M (2006b) Paternal kin bias in the agonistic interventions of adult female rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61:205–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham RW (1980) An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour 75:262–299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zamma K (2014) What makes wild chimpanzees wake up at night? Primates 55:51–57

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler TE (2013) Social effects via olfactory sensory stimuli on reproductive function and dysfunction in cooperative breeding marmosets and tamarins: role of scent in reproductive success. Am J Primatol 75:202–211

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler TE, Epple G, Snowdon CT, Porter TA, Belcher AM, Küderling I (1993) Detection of chemical signals of ovulation in the cotton-top tamarin, Saguinus oedipus. Anim Behav 45:313–322

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the Caribbean Primate Research Center for permission to conduct this study and appreciate the support of the staff of the Cayo Santiago Field Station, especially the census taker Edgar Davila, Julio Resto, Nahirí Rivera, and Giselle Caraballo Cruz for their cooperation throughout the experimental study and during the collection of the DNA samples. Fred Bercovitch, Matt Kessler, John Berard, Michael Krawczak, Peter Nürnberg, and Jörg Schmidtke are acknowledged for starting the genetic database of the Cayo Santiago population in the 1990s. Furthermore, we are grateful to Stefanie Bley and Laura Muniz for improving the genetic database and to Linda Vigilant for providing laboratory access. We also thank Michael Krawczak and Olaf Junge for access to a management program of genetic data (FINDSIRE). Megan Hoffman is kindly acknowledged for her invaluable support during the field period, Kati Drechsel for her assistance during the collection of odor samples, and Doreen Langos for her continuous support in the field. We are further extremely grateful to Roger Mundry and Lars Kulik for their support during the statistical analysis and to Cleve Hicks, Jo Bennett, and the three anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript. The population of Cayo Santiago was supported by grant number 8 P40 OD012217 from the National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), the Office of Research Infrastructure Programs (ORIP) of the National Institutes of Health and the Medical Sciences Campus of the University of Puerto Rico. The content of this publication is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of NCRR or ORIP. The study was conducted within the Jr. Research Group of Primate Kin Selection, an Emmy-Noether Group funded by the German Research Foundation (grant no. WI 1808/1-1, 1-2, 2-1, 3-1 awarded to AW). We thank the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology in Leipzig for their logistic support and for hosting the Jr. Research Group of Primate Kin Selection and the University of Leipzig for graduate funding awarded to SH.

Ethical standards

This study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour/Animal Behavior Society for the use of animals in research, the legal requirements of the country in which the work was carried out, and all institutional guidelines. All research procedures were approved by the CPRC and the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of the University of Puerto Rico (protocol number 4060105).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stefanie Henkel.

Additional information

Communicated by E. Huchard

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

ESM 1

(MPEG 28802 kb)

ESM 2

(MPEG 119566 kb)

ESM 3

(PDF 178 kb)

ESM 4

(PDF 202 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Henkel, S., Lambides, A.R., Berger, A. et al. Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) recognize group membership via olfactory cues alone. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 69, 2019–2034 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-015-2013-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-015-2013-y

Keywords

Navigation