Skip to main content
Log in

No frequency shift in the “D” notes of Carolina chickadee calls in response to traffic noise

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Loud, low-frequency traffic noise can mask songbird vocalizations, and populations of some urban songbird species have shifted the frequency of their vocalizations upward in response. However, the spectral structure of certain vocalization elements may make them resistant to masking, suggesting that species that use these notes could be more successful in areas with high levels of traffic noise. To test this idea, we recorded Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis), whose calls feature “D” notes with an overtone spectral structure, along a traffic noise gradient in Durham and Orange Counties, North Carolina, USA. Frequency parameters of “D” notes did not change with noise level suggesting the possibility that these notes can be communicated effectively in noise, but further investigation is needed to test this hypothesis directly. In addition, we performed a playback experiment demonstrating how the use of spectrograms to measure note frequencies is unreliable, especially when recordings are made in noisy areas. We used an alternative method based on the predictable frequency structure of “D” notes. Our experiment is one of few that address the effects of urban noise on calls produced by both sexes as opposed to song produced only by males during the breeding season. Understanding how vocalizations with different spectral structures may be affected differentially by traffic noise will increase our ability to predict how the expansion of noisy areas may impact songbird community composition in the future.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bartmess-LeVasseur J, Branch CL, Brownin SA, Owens JL, Freeberg TM (2010) Predator stimuli and calling behavior of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis), tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor), and white-breasted nuthatches (Sitta carolinensis). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:1187–1198

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beecher MD (1988) Spectrographic analysis of animal vocalizations: implications of the ‘uncertainty principle’. Bioacoustics 1:187–208

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloomfield LL, Charrier I, Sturdy CB (2004) Note types and coding in parid vocalizations II: the chick-a-dee call of the mountain chickadee (Poecile gambeli). Can J Zool 82:780–793

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloomfield LL, Phillmore LS, Weisman RG, Sturdy CB (2005) Note types and coding in parid vocalizations III: the chick-a-dee call of the Carolina chickadee (Poecile carolinensis). Can J Zool 83:820–833

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer R (1961) Comparative notes on the life history of the Carolina chickadee. Wilson Bull 73:348–373

    Google Scholar 

  • Brumm H (ed) (2013) Animal communication and noise. Animal signals and communication 2. Springer, Berlin

    Google Scholar 

  • Brumm H, Slabbekoorn H (2005) Acoustic communication in noise. Adv Stud Behav 35:151–209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brumm H, Zollinger SA (2013) Avian vocal production in noise. In: Brumm H (ed) Animal communication and noise. Animal signals and communication 2. Springer, Berlin, pp 187–227

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chung DY, Colavita FB (1976) Periodicity pitch perception and its upper frequency limit in cats. Atten Percept Psychophysiol 20:433–437

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cynx J, Shapiro M (1986) Perception of missing fundamental by a species of songbird (Sturnus vulgaris). J Comp Psychol 100:356–360

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ditchkoff SS, Saalfeld ST, Gibson CJ (2006) Animal behavior in urban ecosystems: modifications due to human-induced stress. Urban Ecosyst 9:5–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dowling J, Luther D, Marra P (2012) Comparative effects of urban development and anthropogenic noise on bird songs. Behav Ecol 23:201–209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ficken MS, Ficken RW, Witkin SR (1978) Vocal repertoire of the black-capped chickadee. Auk 95:34–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ficken MS, Hailman ED, Hailman JP (1994) The chick-a-dee call system of the Mexican chickadee. Condor 96:70–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis C, Ortega C, Cruz A (2011) Different behavioural responses to anthropogenic noise by two closely related passerine birds. Biol Lett 7:850–852

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeberg TM (2008) Complexity in the chick-a-dee call of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis): associations of context and signaler behavior to call structure. Auk 125:896–907

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeberg TM (2012) Geographic variation in note composition and use of chick-a-dee calls of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis). Ethology 118:555–565

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaddis P (1979) A comparative analysis of the vocal communication systems of the Carolina chickadee and tufted titmouse. Ph.D. thesis, University of Florida

  • Gil D, Brumm H (2014) Acoustic communication in the urban environment: patterns, mechanisms, and potential consequences of avian song adjustments. In: Gil D, Brumm H (eds) Avian urban ecology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 69–83

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin SE, Podos J (2013) Shift of song frequencies in response to masking tones. Anim Behav 85:435–440

  • Greenewalt CH (1968) Bird song: acoustics and physiology. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Habib L, Bayne EM, Boutin S (2007) Chronic industrial noise affects pairing success and age structure of ovenbirds Seiurus aurocapilla. J Appl Ecol 44:176–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hailman JP (1989) The organization of the major vocalizations in the Paridae. Wilson Bull 101:305–343

    Google Scholar 

  • Halfwerk W, Holleman LJM, Lessells CM, Slabbekoorn H (2011) Negative impact of traffic noise on avian reproductive success. J Appl Ecol 48:210–219

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanna D, Blouin-Demers G, Wilson DR, Mennill DJ (2011) Anthropogenic noise affects song structure in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). J Exp Biol 214:3549–3556

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heffner HE, Whitfield IC (1976) Perception of the missing fundamental by cats. J Acoust Soc Am 59:915–919

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hu Y, Cardoso GC (2010) Which birds adjust the frequency of vocalizations in urban noise? Anim Behav 79:863–867

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krams I, Krama T, Freeberg TM, Kullberg C, Lucas JR (2012) Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: a parid perspective. Philos Trans Roy Soc B 367:1879–1891

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard ML, Horn AG (2008) Does ambient noise affect growth and begging call structure in nestling birds? Behav Ecol 19:502–507

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lowry H, Lill A, Wong BBM (2012) How noisy does a noisy miner have to be? amplitude adjustments of alarm calls in an avian urban adapter. PLoS ONE 7:e29960

  • Lucas JR, Freeberg TM (2007) ‘Information’ and the chick-a-dee call: communicating with a complex vocal system. In: Otter KA (ed) Ecology and behaviour of chickadees and titmice: an integrated approach. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 199–213

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mahurin EJ, Freeberg TM (2009) Chick-a-dee call variation in Carolina chickadees and recruiting flockmates to food. Behav Ecol 20:111–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mammen DL, Nowicki S (1981) Individual differences and within-flock convergence in chickadee calls. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 9:179–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marler P (2006) Bird calls: their potential for behavioral neurobiology. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1016:31–44

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marten K, Marler P (1977) Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization I: temperate habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:271–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaren MA (1976) Vocalizations of the boreal chickadee. Auk 93:451–463

    Google Scholar 

  • Nemeth E, Brumm H (2009) Blackbirds sing higher-pitched songs in cities: adaptation to habitat acoustics or side-effect of urbanization? Anim Behav 78:637–641

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemeth E, Brumm H (2010) Birds and anthropogenic noise: are urban songs adaptive? Am Nat 176:465–475

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nemeth E, Pieretti N, Zollinger SA, Geberzahn N, Partecke J, Miranda AC, Brumm H (2013) Bird song and anthropogenic noise: vocal constraints may explain why birds sing higher-frequency songs in cities. Proc R Soc Lond B 280:1754–1766

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki S (1989) Vocal plasticity in captive black-capped chickadees: the acoustic basis and rate of call convergence. Anim Behav 37:64–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki S, Capranica RR (1986a) Bilateral interaction in vocal production of an oscine bird sound. Science 231:1297–1299

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki S, Capranica RR (1986b) Bilateral syringeal coupling during phonation of a songbird. J Neurosci 6:3595–3610

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osmanski MS, Dooling RJ (2009) The effect of altered auditory feedback on control of vocal production in budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). J Acoust Soc Am 126:911–919

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Partan SR, Fulmer AG, Gounard MAM, Redmond JE (2010) Multimodal alarm behavior in urban and rural gray squirrels studied by means of observation and a mechanical robot. Curr Zool 56:313–326

    Google Scholar 

  • Patricelli GL, Blickley JL (2006) Avian communication in urban noise: causes and consequences of vocal adjustment. Auk 123:639–649

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomp R (1967) Pitch of complex tones. J Acoust Soc Am 41:1526–1533

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pohl NU, Leadbeater E, Slabbekoorn H, Klump GM, Langemann U (2012) Great tits in urban noise benefit from high frequencies in song detection and discrimination. Anim Behav 83:711–721

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potvin DA, Mulder RA (2013) Immediate, independent adjustment of call pitch and amplitude in response to varying background noise by silvereyes (Zosterops lateralis). Behav Ecol 24:1363–1368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potvin DA, Parris KM, Mulder RA (2011) Geographically pervasive effects of urban noise on frequency and syllable rate of songs and calls in silvereyes (Zosterops lateralis). Proc R Soc Lond B 278:2464–2469

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proppe DS, Sturdy CB, St. Clair CC (2011) Flexibility in animal signals facilitates adaptation to rapidly changing environments. PLoS ONE 6:e25413

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proppe DS, Avey MT, Hoeschele M, Moscicki MK, Farrell T, St. Clair CC, Sturdy CB (2012) Black-capped chickadees Poecile atricapillus sing at higher pitches with elevated anthropogenic noise but not with decreasing canopy cover. J Avian Biol 43:325–332

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proppe DS, Sturdy CB, St. Clair CC (2013) Anthropogenic noise decreases urban songbird diversity and may contributes to homogenization. Glob Chang Biol 19:1075–1084

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rabin LA, Greene CM (2002) Changes to acoustic communication systems in human-altered environments. J Comp Psychol 116:137–141

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reijnen R, Foppen R, Braak CT, Thissen J (1995) The effects of car traffic on breeding bird populations in woodland. III Reduction of density in relation to the proximity of main roads. J Appl Ecol 32:187–202

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rheindt FE (2003) The impact of roads on birds: does song frequency play a role in determining susceptibility to noise pollution? J Ornithol 144:295–306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ríos-Chelén AA, Salaberria C, Barbosa I, Macías Garcia C, Gil D (2012) The learning advantage: bird species that learn their song show a tighter adjustment of song to noisy environments than those that do not learn. J Evol Biol 25:2171–2180

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ríos-Chelén AA, Quirós-Guerrero E, Gil D, Garcia CM (2013) Dealing with urban noise: vermilion flycatchers sing longer songs in noisier territories. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67:145–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat JL (2004) Transportation noise issues. In: Kutz M (ed) Handbook of transportation engineering. Vol. 1000. McGraw-Hill, New York, pp 19.1–19.15

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandberg U (1987) Road traffic noise—the influence of the road surface and its characterization. Appl Acoust 21:97–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schuster S, Zollinger SA, Lesku JA, Brumm H (2012) On the evolution of noise-dependent vocal plasticity in birds. Biol Lett 8:913–916

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shofner W (2011) Perception of the missing fundamental by chinchillas in the presence of low-pass masking noise. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 12:101–112

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slabbekoorn H, den Boer-Visser A (2006) Cities change the songs of birds. Curr Biol 16:2326–2331

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith ST (1972) Communication and other social behavior in Parus carolinensis. Nuttall Ornithological Club, Cambridge, MA

  • Soard CM, Ritchison G (2009) Chick-a-dee calls of Carolina chickadees convey information about degree of threat posed by avian predators. Anim Behav 78:1447–1453

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson RWW, Schwarz DWF (1988) Perception of the missing fundamental in nonhuman primates. J Acoust Soc Am 84:560–565

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Traunmüller H, Eriksson E (2000) Acoustic effects of variation in vocal effort by men, women, and children. J Acoust Soc Am 107:3438–3451

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verzijden MN, Ripmeester EAP, Ohms VR, Snelderwaard P, Slabbekoorn H (2010) Immediate spectral flexibility in singing chiffchaffs during experimental exposure to highway noise. J Exp Biol 213:2575–2581

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren PA, Katti M, Ermann M, Brazel A (2006) Urban bioacoustics: it’s not just noise. Anim Behav 71:491–502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiley RH, Richards DG (1982) Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound transmission and signal detection. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds vol. 2. Academic, New York, pp 131–181

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Zollinger SA, Podos J, Nemeth E, Goller F, Brumm H (2012) On the relationship between, and measurement of, amplitude and frequency in birdsong. Anim Behav 84:e1–e9

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Ed Ibarguen and the Washington Duke Inn and Golf Club for providing transportation between sites during data collection, and Sara Childs and the Office of the Duke Forest for permitting access to the Duke Forest for field recordings. We thank Susan Peters for the help in all aspects of the project, especially her help in synthesizing notes for playback and suggesting improvements to the manuscript. Thanks to Stephen Nowicki and his lab at Duke University for their encouragement and suggestions, the S.P.I.C.E. Lab at the University of Central Florida for editing a draft of the manuscript, and Mohamed Noor at Duke University for his prompt and thoughtful feedback on an early draft of the manuscript. Duke University provided logistical and financial support. The project was funded by a Duke University Undergraduate Research Support Grant to MKG.

The Duke University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee approved the project under Protocol No. A237-11-09.

Ethical standards

The experiments performed here comply with the current laws of the United States of America.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rindy C. Anderson.

Additional information

Communicated by H. Brumm

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Grace, M.K., Anderson, R.C. No frequency shift in the “D” notes of Carolina chickadee calls in response to traffic noise. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 69, 253–263 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-014-1838-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-014-1838-0

Keywords

Navigation