Skip to main content
Log in

Antipredator strategies of house finches: are urban habitats safe spots from predators even when humans are around?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Urbanization decreases species diversity, but it increases the abundance of certain species with high tolerance to human activities. The safe-habitat hypothesis explains this pattern through a decrease in the abundance of native predators, which reduces predation risk in urban habitats. However, this hypothesis does not consider the potential negative effects of human-associated disturbance (e.g., pedestrians, dogs, cats). Our goal was to assess the degree of perceived predation risk in house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) through field studies and semi-natural experiments in areas with different levels of urbanization using multiple indicators of risk (flock size, flight initiation distance, vigilance, and foraging behavior). Field studies showed that house finches in more urbanized habitats had a greater tendency to flock with an increase in population density and flushed at larger distances than in less urbanized habitats. In the semi-natural experiment, we found that individuals spent a greater proportion of time in the refuge patch and increased the instantaneous pecking rate in the more urbanized habitat with pedestrians probably to compensate for the lower amount of foraging time. Vigilance parameters were influenced in different ways depending on habitat type and distance to flock mates. Our results suggest that house finches may perceive highly urbanized habitats as more dangerous, despite the lower number of native predators. This could be due to the presence of human activities, which could increase risk or modify the ability to detect predators. House finches seem to adapt to the urban environment through different behavioral strategies that minimize risk.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barnard CJ (1980) Flock feeding and time budgets in the house sparrow (Passer domesticus). Anim Behav 28:295–309

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beale CM, Monaghan P (2004) Behavioural responses to human disturbance: a matter of choice? Anim Behav 68:1065–1069

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp G (2004) Reduced flocking by birds on islands with relaxed predation. Proc R Soc Lon B 271:1039–1042

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckerman AP, Boots M, Gaston KJ (2007) Urban declines and the fear of cats. Anim Conserv 10:320–325

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bednekoff PA, Lima SL (1998) Re-examining safety in numbers: interactions between risk dilution and collective detection depend upon predator targeting behaviour. Proc R Soc Lon B 265:2021–2026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair R (1996) Land use and avian species diversity along an urban gradient. Ecol Appl 6:506–519

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT (2003) Flight initiation distance in birds is dependent on intruder starting distance. J Wildl Manag 67:852–857

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Daniel JC, Evans CS (2006) JWatcher 1.0. An introductory user’s guide. http://www.jwatcher.ucla.edu

  • Brown JS (1988) Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk and competition. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22:37–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caraco T (1979) Time budgeting and group size: a theory. Ecology 60:611–617

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caro T (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooke AS (1980) Observations on how close certain passerine species will tolerate an approaching human in rural and suburban areas. Biol Conserv 18:85–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper WE, Frederick WG (2007) Optimal flight initiation distance. J Theor Biol 244:59–67

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crooks KR (2002) Relative sensitivities of mammalian carnivores to habitat fragmentation. Conserv Biol 16:488–502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crooks KR, Soule ME (1999) Mesopredator release and avifaunal extinctions in a fragmented system. Nature 400:563–566

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dill LM, Houtman R (1989) The influence of distance to refuge on flight initiation distance in the grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis). Can J Zool 67:233–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E (2000) Local and regional effects of pedestrians on forest birds in a fragmented landscape. Condor 102:247–255

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E, Kacelnik A (2004) Information transfer and gain in flocks: the effects of quality and quantity of social information at different neighbor distances. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:502–511

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E, Telleria JL (2000) Effects of human disturbance on spatial and temporal feeding patterns of Blackbird Turdus merula in urban parks in Madrid, Spain. Bird Study 47:13–21

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E, Tran E (2007) Changes in vigilance and foraging behaviour with light intensity and their effects on food intake and predator detection in house finches. Anim Behav 74:1381–1390

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E, Jimenez MD, Lucas E (2001) Bird tolerance to human disturbance in urban parks of Madrid. Spain. Management implications. In: Marzluff JM, Bowman R, Donnelly R (eds) Avian ecology and conservation in an urbanizing world. Kluwer Academic, Norwell, pp 261–275

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E, Siller S, Kacelnik A (2004) Flock density, social foraging, and scanning: an experiment with starlings. Behav Ecol 15:371–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E, Poston R, De Collibus K, Morgan T, Bastain B, Martin C, Jones K, Treminio R (2005) Microhabitat selection and singing behavior patterns of male house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) in urban parks in a heavily urbanized landscape in the Western US. Urban Habitats 3:49–69 ISSN 1541-7115. http://www.urbanhabitats.org/v03n01/finch_pdf.pdf

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Juricic E, Beauchamp G, Bastain B (2007) Group-size and distance-to-neighbour effects on feeding and vigilance in brown-headed cowbirds. Anim Behav 73:771–778

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frid A, Dill L (2002) Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conserv Ecol 6:11 http://www.consecol.org/vol6/iss1/art11

    Google Scholar 

  • Giraldeau LA (1988) The stable group and the determinants of foraging group size. In: Slobodchikoff CN (ed) Ecology of social behavior. Academic, San Diego, pp 33–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart A, Lendrem DW (1984) Vigilance and scanning patterns in birds. Anim Behav 32:1216–1224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill GE (1993) House finch (Carpodacus mexicanus). In: Poole A, Gill F (ed) The birds of North America, No. 46. The Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia. The American Ornithologists’ Union, Washington, D.C.

  • Hilton GM, Cresswell W, Ruxton GD (1999) Intraflock variation in the speed of escape-flight response on attack by an avian predator. Behav Ecol 10:391–395

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Järvinen O, Väisänen RA (1977) Line transect method: a standard for field-work. Pol Ecol Stud 3:11–15

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight RL, Grout DJ, Temple SA (1987) Nest-defense behavior of the American Crow in urban and rural areas. Condor 89:175–177

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotler BP (1997) Patch use by gerbils in a risky environment: manipulating food and safety to test four models. Oikos 78:274–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Lafferty KD (2001) Birds at a Southern California beach: seasonality, habitat use and disturbance by human activity. Biodiv Conserv 10:1949–1962

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus J, Symonds M (1992) Contrasting effects of protective and obstructive cover on avian vigilance. Anim Behav 43:519–521

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL (1990) Protective cover and the use of space: different strategies in finches. Oikos 58:151–158

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL (1998) Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator–prey interactions. Bioscience 48:25–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill L (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:610–640

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Wiebe KL, Dill LM (1987) Protective cover and the use of space by finches: is closer better? Oikos 50:225–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lind J, Cresswell W (2005) Anti-predation behaviour during bird migration; the benefit of studying multiple behavioural dimensions. J Ornithol 147:310–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin J, Luque-Larena JJ, Lopez P (2005) Factors affecting escape behavior of Iberian green frogs (Rana perezi). Can J Zool 83:1189–1194

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marzluff JM (2001) Worldwide urbanization and its effects on birds. In: Marzluff JM, Bowman R, Donnelly R (eds) Avian ecology and conservation in an urbanizing world. Kluwer, Boston, pp 19–38

    Google Scholar 

  • McKinney ML (2006) Urbanization as a major cause of biotic homogenization. Biol Conserv 127:247–260

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Møller AP (2008) Flight distance of urban birds, predation, and selection for urban life. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63:63–75

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan T, Fernández-Juricic E (2007) The effects of predation risk, food abundance, and population size on group size of brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater). Ethology 113:1173–1184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman JA, Recer GM, Zwicker SM, Caraco T (1988) Effects of predation hazard on foraging “constraints”: patch-use strategies in grey squirrels. Oikos 53:93–97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostro LET, Silver SC, Koontz FW, Horwich RH, Brockett R (2001) Shifts in social structure of black howler (Alouatta pigra) groups associated with natural and experimental variation in population density. Int J Primatol 22:733–748

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickett KN, Hik DS, Newsome AE, Pech RP (2005) The influence of predation risk on foraging behaviour of brushtail possums in Australian woodlands. Wildl Res 32:121–130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pöysä H (1994) Group foraging, distance to cover and vigilance in the teal Anas crecca. Anim Behav 48:921–928

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prodon R, Lebreton JD (1981) Breeding avifauna of a Mediterranean succession: the holm oak and cork oak series in the eastern Pyrénées. 1: analysis and modeling of the structure gradient. Oikos 37:21–38

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth TC, Lima SL (2003) Hunting behavior and diet of Cooper’s Hawks: an urban view of small-bird-in-winter paradigm. Condor 105:474–483

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth TC, Lima SL, Vetter WE (2007) The predatory behavior of wintering Accipiter hawks: temporal patterns in activity of predators and prey. Oecologia 152:169–178

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shochat E (2004) Credit or debit? Resource input changes population dynamics of city slicker birds. Oikos 106:622–626

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shochat E, Lerman S, Katti M, Lewis DB (2004) Linking optimal foraging behavior to bird community structure in an urban-desert landscape: field experiments with artificial food patches. Am Nat 164:232–243

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shochat E, Warren PS, Faeth SH, McIntyre NE, Hope D (2006) From patterns to emerging processes in mechanistic urban ecology. Trends Ecol Evol 21:186–191

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sorace A (2002) High density of bird and pest species in urban habitats and the role of predator abundance. Ornis Fennica 79:60–71

    Google Scholar 

  • Stankowich T, Blumstein DT (2005) Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment. Proc R Soc B 272:2627–2634

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stankowich T, Coss RG (2006) Effects of predator behavior and proximity on risk assessment in Columbian black-tailed deer. Behav Ecol 17:246–254

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swennen C, Leopold MF, Debruijn LLM (1989) Time-stressed oystercatchers, Haematopus ostralegus, can increase their intake rate. Anim Behav 38:8–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton JJ, Giraldeau L (1995) Public information cues affect the scrounging decisions of starlings. Anim Behav 49:1617–1626

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomialojc L (1982) Synurbanization of birds and prey–predators relations. In: Luniak M, Pisarski B (eds) Animals in urban environments. Ossolineum, Wrocław, pp 131–137

    Google Scholar 

  • Walther BA, Gosler AG (2001) The effects of food availability and distance to protective cover on the winter foraging behaviour of tits (Aves: Parus). Oecologia 129:312–320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward C, Low BS (1997) Predictors of vigilance for American crows foraging in an urban environment. Wilson Bull 109:481–489

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitfield DP (2003) Redshank Tringa totanus flocking behaviour, distance form cover and vulnerability to sparrow hawk Accipiter nisus predation. J Avian Biol 34:163–169

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whittingham MJ, Evans KL (2004) The effects of habitat structure on predation risk of birds in agricultural landscapes. Ibis 146:210–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wirtz P, Lörscher J (1983) Group sizes of antelopes in an East African park. Behaviour 84:135–155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woods M, McDonald RA, Harris S (2003) Predation of wildlife by domestic cats Felis catus in Great Britain. Mamm Rev 33:174–188

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yalden PE, Yalden DW (1990) Recreational disturbance on breeding golden plovers Pluvialis apricaria. Biol Conserv 51:243–262

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We especially thank the following members of the Behavior and Conservation Laboratory (CSULB) who gladly helped in the different phases of this study: Laura Brandy Adams, Hans Chalco, Karin De Collibus, Tracy Dolan, Rachael Poston, Vanessa Tisdale, Mary Ellen Millard, Shane Oliver, Shelya Jones, Tim Morgan, Elizabeth Tran, Ronald Treminio, Jennie Wong, and Maura Palacios. Robert Cummings and Orange County Vector Control District facilitated access to the birds. Eyal Shochat, James Archie, Judith Brusslan, Dessie Underwood, and two anonymous referees provided valuable comments on earlier versions of the draft. This study was funded by the College of Natural Sciences and Mathematics (CSULB). Experimental protocols were approved by CSULB IACUC (protocol number 223).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Esteban Fernández-Juricic.

Additional information

Communicated by P. Bednekoff

Appendices

Appendix 1. Study areas used and degree of urbanization

The table presents the list of parks in more and less urbanized habitats and their sizes used for this study.

Table 2 List of parks in more and less urbanized habitats and their sizes

We quantified several aspects of these parks to confirm our classification. We obtained information on park area from the Long Beach Parks, Recreation and Marine website (http://www.longbeach.gov/park/facilities/parks) for parks located within Long Beach and from the County of Orange, Harbors, Beaches, and Parks website (http://www.ocparks.com/) for those located in Orange County.

Human disturbance was characterized in the parks where flock surveys were conducted and where FID measurements were taken using different methodologies. We measured the number of pedestrians in the 22 parks listed above by counting the number of people walking and sitting detected within the transects during the flock surveys. In addition, we measured pedestrian rate on the spots in which flight initiation distance had been recorded using two 50-m2 squared plots to better characterize human disturbance in the samples areas: one plot was centered where the focal bird was before flushing, and the other plot was in a random location 50–100 m away from the previous plot. Both pedestrian rate measurements were taken on the same day, but days after we recorded FID. At the center of the 50-m2 plot, during a 10-min period, we counted the number of pedestrians walking and sitting, as well as the number of dogs (leashed and unleashed) that entered the plot and averaged the values from both plots.

We measured vegetation structure in 25-m2 plots at the center of every transect used to survey flocks. We visually estimated (following Prodon and Lebreton 1981): grass cover, bush cover, and tree cover. We measured mean bush and tree height by using a tape meter when physically possible. For taller trees, we estimated height by visually rotating the location of the tip of the tree onto the ground and then measuring the ground distance with a meter tape (±0.05 m; following Fernández-Juricic et al. 2005). We also recorded the number of tree trunks >30-cm dbh), as these represented potential perches for house finches (Fernández-Juricic et al. 2005).

Appendix 2. Description of the experimental procedures to assess scanning and foraging behavior

The semi-natural experiment was carried out from September to November 2005 in two locations separated by 3.87 km: Seal Beach (33° 45′ N, 118° 05′ W; less urbanized) and CSULB campus (33° 46′ N, 118° 06′ W; more urbanized).

Forty-two wild adult house finches were caught and color-banded from two Southern California populations: Seal Beach (33° 45′ N, 118° 05′ W) and Irvine (33° 39′ N, 117° 51′ W). We decided to capture birds from areas that had intermediate degrees of urbanization to avoid habituation effects towards humans. House finches were housed in 0.85 × 0.6 × 0.55-m cages under a 12:12-h light–dark cycle (lights went on 06:00) and were kept in visual and auditory contact with four birds per cage (male and female house finches were housed in the same cages). No breeding behavior was observed during the experimental period. Water and food (finch seed mix, Royal Feeds, Leach Grain and Milling, Co., Downey, CA, USA) were provided ad libitum except during experimental trials and the preceding periods of food deprivation. At the end of the experiments, birds were released at the same locations in which they were captured.

Our experimental setup consisted of three sets of two bottomless circular light wire mesh enclosures (0.5-m diameter, mesh opening 0.06 m, 85% open area) placed in a parallel arrangement. Each enclosure was placed on a round wooden tray and connected by a link (0.3 × 0.18 × 0.18 m) to the other enclosure of the same set (Fig. 4). One of the enclosures (hereafter, foraging patch) contained 3 cm of sawdust and 5.00 ± 0.05 g of hidden sunflower seeds. The other enclosure (hereafter, refuge patch) had synthetic foliage placed around the top of the cage and one synthetic bush with the base in the center of the refuge. We placed one bird in each set of enclosures during the experiment, totaling three birds per trial. We considered the individual in the central set as the focal animal to minimize edge effects.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Semi-natural experimental design testing habitat type and neighbor distance. Each enclosure consists of a foraging patch connected by a corridor to a refuge patch. This example shows the 2 m (far) neighbor distance treatment

We manipulated two different treatments: habitat type (three levels: less urbanized without pedestrians, more urbanized with pedestrians, and more urbanized without pedestrians) and neighbor distance (two levels: close, 0-m separation between the sets of enclosures, and far, 2-m separation between the sets of enclosures, Fig. 4). Thus, each focal bird was exposed to six different treatments (one per day) in a random order. Trials were performed in the mornings from 07:00 to 12:00 but not during high winds or rain.

The treatments in the more urbanized habitat were conducted at CSULB campus on a grassy area 20 m from a pathway frequented by pedestrians and 15 m from a large old pine tree and a four-story campus building. For the more urbanized habitat without pedestrian treatment, we surrounded the experimental area with a 2.0-m-high fence and black tarp to screen out visual stimuli (e.g., pedestrians), but the building was still in the visual field of animals. The purpose of this treatment was to obtain the baseline behavior of birds without the influence of pedestrians or habitat openness that would trigger changes in vigilance to monitor for a potential predator. The downside is that it created a visual obstruction effect that may have blocked the need for vigilance.

For the more urbanized habitat with pedestrians treatment, we used the same location without the fence so that birds were exposed to pedestrians as well as an assistant walking in a parallel path 9 m away from the enclosures to simulate the presence of humans. The less urbanized habitat treatment was performed at Seal Beach in an area without a fence. This experimental site was not near any human structures or trees and did not have any pedestrian traffic. The vegetation consisted of native bushy plants and bare ground. The differences in habitat structure between these sites mirrored the ones found between more and less urbanized parks.

Out of the 42 birds, 14 served as focals (seven males and seven females) and were used in a total of 84 trials. The remaining 28 non-focal birds (conspecifics) were assigned to a focal bird and went through all trials with the same focal individuals but were randomly assigned to the right and left sets of enclosures. All birds were food-deprived for 2 h prior to the experiments and transported to the CSULB experimental site by walking and to the Seal Beach location by car. Animals were then placed in the shade until their trial time. During transport, and, while awaiting their trial, they were provided water ad libitum.

The use of a car as a means of transporting the animals to the less urbanized habitat to run the semi-natural experiment could have produced changes in their behavior. To test this possibility, we ran another experiment at only the CSULB campus site, in which house finches experienced two treatments. In the first treatment (driving), birds were driven for 10 min and then placed back in their aviary cages for 30 min before they were used in the experiments. This treatment was conducted with the same car used during the experiments and replicated the less urbanized treatment procedures in the main experiment. In the second treatment (non-driving), birds were not driven at all but were walked to the experimental site replicating the more urbanized treatment procedures in the main experiment. The same three sets of enclosures were placed 0.5 m apart (Fig. 4), with one bird in each under similar conditions as described in the “Materials and methods.” We screened out visual stimuli by fencing around the cages in all treatments. We recorded the foraging and scanning behavior of eight focal birds during 10 min, with each focal being exposed to each treatment. The birds used in this experiment were different from those used in the one presented in the main text.

None of the secondary factors (temperature vs. food deprivation time, r = 0.24, P = 0.255; temperature vs. body condition, r = 0.30, P = 0.150; food deprivation time vs. body condition, r = 0.04, P = 0.840) was correlated among each other. Using general linear models (repeated-measures design), we found that the proportion of time spent in the refuge did not vary between treatments (driving, 0.03 ± 0.01; non-driving, 0.02 ± 0.01; F 1, 4 = 1.39, P = 0.303), controlling for temperature (F 1, 4 = 0.00, P = 0.968), food deprivation time (F 1, 4 = 0.07, P = 0.811), and body condition (F 1, 4 = 2.82, P = 0.168). Scan rate (driving, 18.19 ± 2.25 events per minute; non-driving, 16.83 ± 2.25 events per minute; F 1, 4 = 0.38, P = 0.572) and scan bout duration (driving, 2.25 ± 0.29 s; non-driving, 2.83 ± 0.29 s; F 1, 4 = 2.12, P = 0.219) did not differ between treatments, controlling for the three secondary factors (scan rate: temperature, F 1, 4 = 1.51, P = 0.287; food deprivation time, F 1, 4 = 1.26, P = 0.324; body condition, F 1, 4 = 0.11, P = 0.754; scan bout duration: temperature, F 1, 4 = 5.60, P = 0.077; food deprivation time, F 1, 4 = 3.15, P = 0.151; body condition, F 1, 4 = 1.14, P = 0.346). Finally, there was no difference in pecking rates between treatments (driving, 21.75 ± 2.86 events per minute; non-driving, 18.28 ± 2.86 events per minute; F 1, 4 = 1.29, P = 0.321), controlling for the secondary factors (temperature, F 1, 4 = 6.28, P = 0.066; food deprivation time, F 1, 4 = 5.16, P = 0.0.86; body condition, F 1, 4 = 0.09, P = 0.776). Therefore, we conclude that driving the birds to conduct the less urbanized treatment would not substantially affect their behavior.

During the main experiment, the focal individual was recorded for 20 min using a Sony DCR-TRV38 digital video camera starting from the time it began pecking in the foraging patch. The camera was placed on a tripod 3 m from the focal’s set of enclosures, which allowed us to record its scanning and foraging behavior while in the foraging patch and its use of the refuge patch. We recorded behavior using an event-recording program (JWatcher 1.0; Blumstein et al. 2006). When birds were in the foraging patch, they could be on the ground or hanging on the cage wall. We only focused on their behavior on the ground as a proxy of normal foraging behavior. House finches alternated bouts of food-seeking behavior with their heads down and bouts of scanning behavior with their heads up. We recorded the number of head-up scanning events and their duration, including head-up food-handling behavior as they use it for vigilance (Fernández-Juricic and Tran 2007). While house finches were head-down, we recorded the number and duration of pecking events. We calculated the following dependent variables: head-up scan bout duration, head-up scan rate, and instantaneous pecking rate (number of pecking events per unit time head-down). We also recorded and calculated the proportion of time the focal bird spent in the refuge patch. Video analysis was performed by AV after extensive self-training. At the time of recording, there was less than a 5% difference between two scorings of the same tape. For every trial, we recorded several potential confounding factors: food deprivation time, ambient temperature, wind speed, and focal body condition (body mass/wing length).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Valcarcel, A., Fernández-Juricic, E. Antipredator strategies of house finches: are urban habitats safe spots from predators even when humans are around?. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63, 673–685 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0701-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0701-6

Keywords

Navigation