Skip to main content
Log in

Butterfly effects in mimicry? Combining signal and taste can twist the relationship of Müllerian co-mimics

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Müllerian co-mimics are aposematic species that resemble each other; sharing a warning signal is thought to be mutually beneficial for the co-mimics by reducing per capita predation risk. In Batesian mimicry, edible mimics avoid predation by resembling an aposematic model species. The protection of both the model and the mimic is weakened when the mimics are abundant compared to the models. The quasi-Batesian view suggests that defended (Müllerian) co-mimics, when unequal in their defences, could also show a Batesian-like trend of increasing mortality with increasing abundance of a less defended “mimic”. We manipulated frequencies of unequally distasteful artificial co-mimics that were prey for great tits. The co-mimics had different signals (imperfect mimicry) but were equally preferred by the birds when palatable. Unexpectedly, when unpalatable, one of the signals was easier for the birds to learn to avoid. Consequently, during predator learning, the signal design of the prey strongly affected mortality of the co-mimics; there was an interaction between the signal and frequency treatments, but increasing the frequency of a less defended “mimic” did not increase co-mimic mortalities as predicted. In contrast, in a memory test that followed, the effect of signal design disappeared; if the birds had experienced high frequency of “mimics” during learning, co-mimic mortalities did subsequently increase. Since the effect of co-mimic frequencies on mortalities changed depending on the signal design of the prey and predator experience, the results suggest that mimetic relationship may be an unpredictable interplay of several factors in addition to taste and abundance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alatalo RV, Mappes J (1996) Tracking the evolution of warning signals. Nature 382:708–710

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Alcock J (1970) Punishment levels and the response of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus) to three kinds of artificial seeds. Anim Behav 18:592–599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates HW (1862) Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon Valley Lepidoptera: Heliconidae. Transact Linn Soc Lond 23:495–556

    Google Scholar 

  • Beatty CD, Beirinckx K, Sherratt TN (2004) The evolution of Müllerian mimicry in multispecies communities. Nature 431:63–67

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers MD, Farley S (1990) The behaviour of grey jays, Perisoreus canadensis, towards palatable and unpalatable Lepidoptera. Anim Behav 39:699–705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyden TC (1976) Butterfly palatability and mimicry: experiments with Ameiva lizards. Evolution 30:73–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brower JvZ (1958) Experimental studies of mimicry in some North American butterflies Part II Battus philenor and Papilio trolius, P. polyxenes and P. glaucus. Evolution 12:123–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brower JvZ (1960) Experimental studies of mimicry. IV. The reactions of starlings to different proportions of models and mimics. Am Nat XCIV:271–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brower LP, Brower JvZ, Collins CT (1963) Experimental studies of mimicry. 7. Relative palatability and Müllerian mimicry among neotropical butterflies of the subfamily Heliconiinae. Zoologica 48:65–84

    Google Scholar 

  • Brower LP, Brower JvZ, Westcott PW (1960) Experimental studies of mimicry. 5. The reactions of toads (Bufo bufo) to bumblebees (Bombus americanorum) and their robberfly mimics (Mallophora bomboides), with a discussion of aggressive mimicry. Am Nat XCIV:343–355

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brower LP, McEvoy PB, Williamson KL, Flannery MA (1972) Variation in cardiac glycoside content of monarch butterflies from natural populations in eastern North America. Science 177:426–428

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brower LP, Ryerson WN, Coppinger LL, Glazier SC (1968) Ecological chemistry and the palatability spectrum. Science 161:1349–1350

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown KSJ, Benson WW (1974) Adaptive polymorphism associated with multiple Müllerian mimicry in Heliconius numata (Lepid. Nymph.). Biotropica 6:205–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darst CR, Cummings ME (2006) Predator learning favours mimicry of a less-toxic model in poison frogs. Nature 440:208–211

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eggenberger F, Rowell-Rahier M (1992) Genetic component of variation in chemical defence of Oreina gloriosa (Coleoptera: Chrysomelidae). J Chem Ecol 18:1375–1387

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher RA (1958) The genetical theory of natural selection, 2nd revised edn. Denver, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Franks DW, Noble J (2004) Batesian mimics influence mimicry ring evolution. Proc R Soc Lond B 271:191–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert FS (2005) The evolution of imperfect mimicry. In: Fellowes MDE, Holloway GJ, Roitt J (eds) Insect evolutionary ecology: Proceedings of the Royal Entomological Society’s 22nd symposium, University of Reading. CABI, UK, pp 231–288

    Google Scholar 

  • Huheey JE (1976) Studies of warning coloration and mimicry. VII. Evolutionary consequences of Batesian–Müllerian spectrum: a model for Müllerian mimicry. Evolution 30:86–93

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huheey JE (1988) Mathematical models of mimicry. Am Nat 131:S22–S41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ihalainen E, Lindström L, Mappes J (2007) Investigating Müllerian mimicry: predator learning and variation in prey defences. J Evol Biol 20:780–791

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ihalainen E, Lindström L, Mappes J, Puolakkanen, S (2008) Can experienced birds select for Müllerian mimicry? Behavioral Ecology. DOI 10.1093/beheco/arm151

  • Joron M, Mallet JLB (1998) Diversity in mimicry: paradox or paradigm. Trends Ecol Evol 13:461–466

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapan DD (2001) Three-butterfly system provides a field test of müllerian mimicry. Nature 409:338–340

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kokko H, Mappes J, Lindström L (2003) Alternative prey can change model-mimic dynamics between paratism and mutualism. Ecology Letters 6:1068–1076

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindström L, Alatalo RV, Lyytinen A, Mappes J (2001) Strong antiapostatic selection against novel rare aposematic prey. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:9181–9184

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindström L, Alatalo RV, Lyytinen A, Mappes J (2004) The effect of alternative prey on the dynamics of imperfect Batesian and Müllerian mimicries. Evolution 58:1294–1302

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindström L, Alatalo RV, Mappes J (1997) Imperfect Batesian mimicry—the effects of the frequency and the distastefulness of the model. Proc R Soc Lond B 264:149–153

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindström L, Lyytinen A, Mappes J, Ojala K (2006) Relative importance of taste and visual appearance for predator education in Müllerian mimicry. Anim Behav 72:323–333

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mallet JLB (1990) Is mimicry theory unpalatable? Trends Ecol Evol 5:344–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mallet JLB (1999) Causes and consequences of a lack of coevolution in Müllerian mimicry. Evol Ecol 13:777–806

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mallet JLB, Barton NH (1989) Strong natural selection in a warning-color hybrid zone. Evolution 43:421–431

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall GAK (1908) On diaposematism with reference to some limitations of the Müllerian hypothesis of mimicry. Trans Entomol Soc Lond 1908:93–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller F (1879) Ituna and Thyridia: a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies. Transactions of the Entomological Society of London 1879:XX–XXIX

    Google Scholar 

  • Nijhout F (2003) Polymorphic mimicry in Papilio dardanus: mosaic dominance, big effects, and origins. Evolut Develop 5:579–592

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nonacs P (1985) Foraging in a dynamic mimicry complex. Am Nat 126:165–180

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owen RE, Owen ARG (1984) Mathematical paradigm for mimicry: recurrent sampling. J Theor Biol 109:217–247

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfenning DW, Harcombe WR, Pfenning KS (2001) Frequency-dependent Batesian mimicry Predators avoid look-alikes of venomous snakes only when the real thing is around. Nature 410:323

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulton EB (1890) The colours of animals. Their meaning and use. Especially considered in the case of insects. Kegan Paul, Trench, Trübner, & Co., London

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe C, Lindström L, Lyytinen A (2004) The importance of pattern similarity between Müllerian mimics in predator avoidance learning. Proc R Soc Lond B 271:407–413

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowland HM, Ihalainen E, Lindström L, Mappes J, Speed MP (2007) Co-mimics have a mutualistic relationship despite unequal defences. Nature 448:64–67

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sargent TD (1995) On the relative acceptabilities of local butterflies and moths to local birds. J Lepid Soc 49:148–162

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheppard PM (1959) The evolution of mimicry; a problem in ecology and genetics. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 24:131–140

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sherratt TN, Speed MP, Ruxton GD (2004) Natural selection on unpalatable species imposed by state-dependent foraging behaviour. J Theor Biol 228:217–226

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skelhorn J, Rowe C (2004) Tasting the difference: do multiple defence chemicals interact in Müllerian mimicry. Proc R Soc Lond B 272:339–345

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skelhorn J, Rowe C (2006a) Prey palatability influences predator learning and memory. Anim Behav 71:111–1118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skelhorn J, Rowe C (2006b) Taste-rejection by predators and the evolution of unpalatability in prey. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:550–555

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skelhorn J, Rowe C (2007) Predators’ toxin burdens influence their strategic decisions to eat toxic prey. Curr Biol 17:1479–1483

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Speed MP (1993) Müllerian mimicry and the psychology of predation. Anim Behav 45:571–580

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speed MP (1999) Batesian, quasi-Batesian or Müllerian mimicry? Theory and data in mimicry research. Evol Ecol 13:755–776

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speed MP, Alderson NJ, Hardman C, Ruxton GD (2000) Testing Müllerian mimicry: an experiment with wild birds. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:725–731

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Speed MP, Turner JRG (1999) Learning and memory in mimicry: II. Do we understand the mimicry spectrum? Biol J Linn Soc 67:281–312

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner JRG (1987) The evolutionary dynamics of batesian and muellerian mimicry: similarities and differences. Ecol Entomol 12:81–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner JRG, Kearney EP, Exton LS (1984) Mimicry and the Monte Carlo predator: the palatability spectrum and the origins of mimicry. Biol J Linn Soc 23:247–268

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner JRG, Speed MP (1996) Learning and memory in mimicry. I. Simulations of laboratory experiments. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 351:1157–1170

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank Helinä Nisu for taking loving care of the birds, the staff of Konnevesi Research Station for taking care of us and Mike Speed and two reviewers for comments. Hannah Rowland kindly corrected the language. Great tits 458815J, 458828J and all from 458876J to 458892J and from 666964J to 666989J, bless them, are warmly acknowledged. The study was financed by the Academy of Finland (project nos. 779874, 7208265 and Centre of Excellence in Evolutionary Ecology). All experiments comply with Finnish laws.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eira Ihalainen.

Additional information

Communicated by D. Gwynne

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ihalainen, E., Lindström, L., Mappes, J. et al. Butterfly effects in mimicry? Combining signal and taste can twist the relationship of Müllerian co-mimics. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62, 1267–1276 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0555-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0555-y

Keywords

Navigation