Skip to main content
Log in

Haptic feedback attenuates illusory bias in pantomime-grasping: evidence for a visuo-haptic calibration

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Relative visual information has been shown to mediate grasping responses directed to an area previously occupied by a target object (i.e., pantomime-grasping) and is an information type functionally distinct from the absolute visual information supporting naturalistic grasping (i.e., grasping a physical target). Pantomime- and naturalistic grasps differ not only in terms of their visual properties, but also because the former lacks physical interaction with a target object (i.e., no-haptic feedback). The absence of haptic feedback may represent a reason why pantomime- and naturalistic grasps differ. To address this issue, participants completed pantomime-grasps to objects embedded in fins-in and fins-out configurations of the Müller–Lyer (ML) illusion following a 2000-ms visual delay when haptic feedback was unavailable (H− condition), and when experimentally induced (H+ condition). In particular, in the H+ condition the experimenter placed a physical target object between participants’ thumb and forefinger after they completed their grasping response. H− and H+ conditions were performed when online vision was available (i.e., Experiment 1) and when withdrawn (i.e., Experiment 2). If haptic feedback influences grasping, then the absolute information afforded from physically touching a target object (i.e., the H+ condition) should result in aperture metrics that are refractory—or attenuated—to the relative properties of the ML figures. Grip apertures in H− and H+ conditions were “tricked” in a direction consistent with the perceptual effects of the ML illusion; however, Experiment 2 showed that illusory effects were attenuated in the H+ condition. In other words, responses without online vision showed evidence of a visuo-haptic calibration. These results provide convergent evidence that haptic and visual feedback play a salient role in considering the extant literature’s documented report of kinematic differences between pantomime- and naturalistic grasps.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Schenk (2012) did not use the term “pantomime-grasp” to refer to his condition(s) wherein haptic feedback was unavailable. In particular, Schenk referred to his condition wherein H- trials were completed in a separate block as “Grasping without haptic feedback (task 4),” and the condition in which H− and H + trials were interleaved as “Grasping with intermittent haptic feedback (task 5).” The tasks above match the operational definition for remembered target pantomime-grasps provided in the present Introduction. Further, in personal communication Thomas Schenk stated “I would agree that task 4 is a pantomime task, but so is (in my view) task 5.”

  2. Davarpanah Jazi and Heath (2016) showed that H+ trials performed in a blocked—but not random—feedback schedule adhered to Weber’s law. It was proposed that trial-to-trial knowledge of haptic feedback availability serves as the optimal environment to support a visuo-haptic calibration.

  3. For consistency, we present t tests for our between-experiment comparisons. Notably, however, Fig. 3 presents means and 95% between-participant confidence intervals and therefore provides a graphical basis for determining reliable between-experiment differences. For example, in the H− condition (and also the H+ condition) the absence of overlap between Experiments 1 and 2 error bars demonstrates that the magnitude of the illusory effect was reliably larger in the latter experiment (for review of graphical analyses see Cumming 2013).

  4. Some work has proposed that a corrected illusion effect accounting for condition-based differences in physical response functions (i.e., the physical change in the size of a target object) is necessary to determine illusion effects in reaching and grasping (e.g., Hesse et al. 2016). For comparison with other work, Table 3 presents the mean corrected illusion effects for H− and H+ conditions for Experiments 1 and 2 and presents the correlation coefficient for H− and H+ condition corrected illusion effects.

  5. Two different target object sizes (50 and 70 mm) were used in Experiments 1 and 2, and they were randomly presented on a trial-by-trial basis. The random presentation was used so that in the H+ condition participants did not simply rely on haptic feedback provided on trial N–1 to specify grip aperture on trial N. It is, however, possible that the limited number of target object sizes used here resulted in some “haptic learning” and served to support a visuo-haptic calibration.

References

  • Bingham G, Coats R, Mon-Williams M (2007) Natural prehension in trials without haptic feedback but only when calibration is allowed. Neuropsychologia 45:288–294

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruno N, Franz VH (2009) When is grasping affected by the Müller–Lyer illusion? A quantitative review. Neuropsychologia 47:1421–1433

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryden MP (1977) Measuring handedness with questionnaires. Neuropsychologia 15:617–624

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cavina-Pratesi C, Kuhn G, Ietswaart M, Milner AD (2011) The magic grasp: motor expertise in deception. PLoS ONE 6:e16568

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Conti P, Beaubaton D (1980) Role of structured visual field and visual reafference in accuracy of pointing movements. Percept Mot Skills 50:239–244

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cumming G (2013) Understanding the new statistics: effect sizes, confidence intervals, and meta-analysis. Routledge, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M (2014) Weber’s law in tactile grasping and manual estimation: feedback-dependent evidence for functionally distinct processing streams. Brain Cognit 86:32–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M (2016) Pantomime-grasping: advance knowledge of haptic feedback availability supports an absolute visuo-haptic calibration. Front Hum Neurosci 10:197

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Davarpanah Jazi S, Hosang S, Heath M (2015a) Memory delay and haptic feedback influence the dissociation of tactile cues for perception and action. Neuropsychologia 71:91–100

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davarpanah Jazi S, Yau M, Westwood DA, Heath M (2015b) Pantomime-grasping: the ‘return’ of haptic feedback supports the absolute specification of object size. Exp Brain Res 233:2029–2040

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429–433

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan JR, Bowman MC, Johansson RS (2006) Control strategies in object manipulation tasks. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:650–659

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fukui T, Inui T (2013) How vision affects kinematic properties of pantomimed prehension movements. Front Psychol 4:44

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ganel T, Chajut E, Algom D (2008) Visual coding for action violates fundamental psychophysical principles. Curr Biol 18:R599–R601

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ganel T, Freud E, Chajut E, Algom D (2012) Accurate visuomotor control below the perceptual threshold of size discrimination. PLoS ONE 7:e36253

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA (2011) Transforming vision into action. Vision Res 51:1567–1587

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA, Jakobson LS, Keillor JM (1994) Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia 32:1159–1178

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heath M, Rival C, Binsted G (2004) Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the Müller–Lyer illusion. Exp Brain Res 158:378–384

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heath M, Rival C, Westwood DA, Neely K (2005) Time course analysis of closed- and open-loop grasping of the Müller–Lyer illusion. J Mot Behav 37:179–185

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heath M, Davarpanah Jazi S, Holmes SA (2015) An inverse grip starting posture gives rise to time-dependent adherence to Weber’s law: a reply to Ganel et al. (2014). J. Vis 15:1

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hesse C, Franz VH, Schenk T (2016) Pointing and antipointing in Müller-lyer figures: why illusion effects need to be scaled. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 42:90–102

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes SA, Lohmus J, McKinnon S, Mulla A, Heath M (2013) Distinct visual cues mediate aperture shaping for grasping and pantomime-grasping tasks. J Mot Behav 45:431–439

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hosang S, Chan J, Davarpanah Jazi S, Heath M (2016) Grasping a 2D object: terminal haptic feedback supports an absolute visuo-haptic calibration. Exp Brain Res 234:945–954

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hu Y, Goodale MA (2000) Grasping after a delay shifts size-scaling from absolute to relative metrics. J Cogn Neurosci 12:856–868

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys GW, Price CJ, Riddoch MJ (1999) From objects to names: a cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychol Res 62:118–130

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ishai A, Ungerleider LG, Martin A, Schouten JL, Haxby JV (1999) Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:9379–9384

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • James TW, Culham J, Humphrey GK, Milner AD, Goodale MA (2003) Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: an fMRI study. Brain 126:2463–2475

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krigolson O, Heath M (2004) Background visual cues and memory-guided reaching. Hum Mov Sci 23:861–877

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krigolson O, Clark N, Heath M, Binsted G (2007) The proximity of visual landmarks impacts reaching performance. Spat Vis 20:317–336

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL (2009) Haptic perception: a tutorial. Atten Percept Psychophys 71:1439–1459

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus GR, Masson MEJ (1994) Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychon Bull Rev 1:476–490

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milner AD, Ganel T, Goodale MA (2012) Does grasping in patient D.F. depend on vision? Trends Cognit Sci 16:256–257

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obhi SS, Goodale MA (2005) The effects of landmarks on the performance of delayed and real-time pointing movements. Exp Brain Res 167:335–344

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ronsse R, Miall RC, Swinnen SP (2009) Multisensory integration in dynamical behaviors: maximum likelihood estimation across bimanual skills learning. J Neurosci 29:8419–8428

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schenk T (2012) No dissociation between perception and action in patient DF when haptic feedback is withdrawn. J Neurosci 32:2013–2017

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smeets JB, Brenner E (1999) A new view on grasping. Mot Control 3:237–271

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Velay JL, Beaubaton D (1986) Influence of visual context on pointing movement accuracy. Cah Psychol Cognit 6:447–456

    Google Scholar 

  • Westwood DA, Chapman CD, Roy EA (2000a) Pantomimed actions may be controlled by the ventral visual stream. Exp Brain Res 130:545–548

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westwood DA, Heath M, Roy EA (2000b) The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension. Exp Brain Res 134:456–463

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westwood DA, McEachern T, Roy EA (2001) Delayed grasping of a Müller–Lyer figure. Exp Brain Res 141:166–173

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitwell RL, Buckingham G (2013) Reframing the action and perception dissociation in DF: haptics matters, but how? J Neurophysiol 109:621–624

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whitwell RL, Milner AD, Cavina-Pratesi C, Byrne CM, Goodale MA (2014) DF’s visual brain in action: the role of tactile cues. Neuropsychologia 55:41–50

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269:1880–1882

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and Major Academic Development Fund and Faculty Scholar Awards from the University of Western Ontario.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Heath.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chan, J., Heath, M. Haptic feedback attenuates illusory bias in pantomime-grasping: evidence for a visuo-haptic calibration. Exp Brain Res 235, 1041–1051 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4860-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4860-9

Keywords

Navigation