Skip to main content
Log in

Active error corrections enhance adaptation to a visuo-motor rotation

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In two experiments we studied the role of active movements for adaptation to a visuo-motor rotation by way of adding external forces. In the first experiment we compared practice with target guidance and path guidance with a no-guidance control condition. With target guidance the arm was driven to the target on a straight path, whereas with path guidance active movements were required which were driven to the correct straight path of the hand whenever there was a deviation. During practice target guidance resulted in faster movements with smaller initial direction errors than in the other two conditions. However, in subsequent visual open-loop tests the adaptive shifts turned out to be smaller after both target-guidance and path-guidance practice than after no-guidance practice. In the second experiment resistive path guidance during practice was compared with a no-guidance control condition. With resistive path guidance the hand was driven away from the correct straight path whenever there was a deviation. Thus, corrections required active movements and could not be passive as with assistive path guidance in the first experiment. During practice resistive path guidance resulted in longer movement time than in the no-guidance group. Adaptive shifts, as assessed in subsequent open-loop tests, were not different between the two groups. According to these findings adaptation to a visuo-motor rotation is driven by active error corrections.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abeele S, Bock O (2001a) Mechanisms for sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input. Exp Brain Res 139:248–253

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Abeele S, Bock O (2001b) Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: different mechanisms for small versus large rotations. Exp Brain Res 140:407–410

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Abeele S, Bock O (2003) Transfer of sensorimotor adaptation between different movement categories. Exp Brain Res 148:128–132

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adams JA (1971) A closed-loop theory of motor learning. J Mot Behav 3:111–155

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Bradley DC, Xing J (1997) Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Ann Rev Neurosci 20:303–330

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baily JS (1972) Adaptation to prisms: do proprioceptive changes mediate adapted behavior without ballistic arm movements? Quart J Exp Psychol 24:8–20

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bedford FL (1993) Perceptual and cognitive spatial learning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 19:517–530

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bedford FL (1995) Constraints on perceptual learning: objects and dimensions. Cognition 54:253–297

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buch ER, Young S, Contreras-Vidal JL (2003) Visuomotor adaptation in normal aging. Learn Mem 10:55–63

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burdet E, Osu R, Franklin DW, Milner TE, Kawato M (2001) The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance. Nature 414:446–449

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cesqui B, Aliboni S, Mazzoleni S, Carrozza MC, Posteraro F, Micera S (2009) On the use of divergent force fields in robot-mediated neurorehabilitation. In: Biomedical robotics and biomechatronics, BioRob 2008. IEEE, Scottsdale, AZ, pp 854–861. doi:10.1109/BIOROB.2008.4762927

  • Clower DM, Boussaoud D (2000) Selective use of perceptual recalibration versus visuomotor skill acquisition. J Neurophysiol 84:2703–2708

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Coren S (1966) Adaptation to prismatic displacement as a function of the amount of available information. Psychon Sci 4:407–408

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham HA (1989) Aiming error under transformed spatial mappings suggests a structure for visual-motor maps. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15:493–506

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Feygin D, Keehner M, Tendick F (2002) Haptic guidance: experimental evaluation of a haptic training method for a perceptual motor skill. In: Proceedings of the 10th international symposium on haptic interfaces for virtual environment and teleoperator system (Haptics 2002). IEEE computer society, Orlando, pp 40–47

  • Franklin DW, Liaw G, Milner TE, Osu R, Burdet E, Kawato M (2007) Endpoint stiffness of the arm is directionally tuned to instability in the environment. J Neurosci 27:7705–7716

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ghilardi MF, Gordon J, Ghez C (1995) Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas. J Neurophysiol 73:2535–2539

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guedon O, Gauthier G, Cole J, Vercher J-L, Blouin J (1998) Adaptation in visuomotor tracking depends on intact proprioception. J Mot Behav 30:234–248

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Held R, Hein A (1958) Adaptation to disarranged hand-eye coordination contigent upon reafferent stimulation. Percept Mot Skills 8:87–90

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H, Hegele M (2008) Adaptation to direction-dependent visuo-motor rotations and its decay in younger and older adults. Acta Psychol 127:369–381

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heuer H, Hegele M, Sülzenbrück S (in press) Implicit and explicit adjustments to extrinsic visuo-motor transformations and their age-related changes. Hum Mov Sci. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2010.07.004

  • Ingram HA, van Donkelaar P, Cole J, Vercher J-L, Gauthier GM, Miall RC (2000) The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task. Exp Brain Res 132:114–126

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Keele SW, Ivry RB, Mayr U, Hazeltine E, Heuer H (2003) The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. Psychol Rev 110:316–339

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kitazawa S, Kimura T, Uka T (1997) Prism adaptation of reaching movements: specificity for the velocity of reaching. J Neurosci 17:1481–1492

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (2000) Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J Neurosci 20:8916–8924

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni P, Krakauer JW (2006) An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. J Neurosci 26:3642–3645

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pipereit K, Bock O, Vercher J-L (2006) The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation. Exp Brain Res 174:45–52

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reinkensmeyer DJ, Patton JL (2009) Can robots help the learning of skilled actions? Exerc Sport Sci Rev 37:43–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saijo N, Gomi H (2010) Multiple motor learning strategies in visuomotor rotation. PLoS ONE 5(2):e9399

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt RA, Lee T (1999) Motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis, 3rd edn. Human Kinetics Publishers, Champaign

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidtke V, Heuer H (1997) Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning. Psychol Res 60:53–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shadmehr R, Smith MA, Krakauer JW (2010) Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu Rev Neurosci 33:89–108

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sülzenbrück S, Heuer H (2009) Functional independence of explicit and implicit motor adjustments. Conscious Cogn 18:145–159

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor JA, Ivry RB (2011) Flexible cognitive strategies during motor learning. PloS Comput Biol 7(3):e1001096

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB (1972) The effect of experienced limb identity upon adaptation to simulated displacement of the visual field. Percept Psychophys 12:453–456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB (1974) Research on adaptation to rearranged vision : 1966–1974. Perception 3:367–392

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Welch RB, Rhoades RW (1969) The manipulation of informational feedback and its effects upon prism adaptation. Can J Psychol 23:415–428

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Kawato M (1998) Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Netw 11:1317–1329

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research reported in this paper was supported by the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme, Grant Agreement Number 231724 (HUMOUR). We thank Johanna Maag, Tuğba Özcan, and Philipp Quiring for their support in running the experiments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Herbert Heuer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Heuer, H., Rapp, K. Active error corrections enhance adaptation to a visuo-motor rotation. Exp Brain Res 211, 97–108 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2656-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2656-5

Keywords

Navigation