Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Locomotor behaviour of children while navigating through apertures

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

During everyday locomotion, we encounter a range of obstacles requiring specific motor responses; a narrow aperture which forces us to rotate our shoulders in order to pass through is one example. In adults, the decision to rotate their shoulders is body scaled (Warren and Whang in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:371–383, 1987), and the movement through is temporally and spatially tailored to the aperture size (Higuchi et al. in Exp Brain Res 175:50–59, 2006; Wilmut and Barnett in Hum Mov Sci 29:289–298, 2010). The aim of the current study was to determine how 8-to 10-year-old children make action judgements and movement adaptations while passing through a series of five aperture sizes which were scaled to body size (0.9, 1.1, 1.3, 1.5 and 1.7 times shoulder width). Spatial and temporal characteristics of movement speed and shoulder rotation were collected over the initial approach phase and while crossing the doorway threshold. In terms of making action judgements, results suggest that the decision to rotate the shoulders is not scaled in the same way as adults, with children showing a critical ratio of 1.61. Shoulder angle at the door could be predicted, for larger aperture ratios, by both shoulder angle variability and lateral trunk variability. This finding supports the dynamical scaling model (Snapp-Childs and Bingham in Exp Brain Res 198:527–533, 2009). In terms of movement adaptations, we have shown that children, like adults, spatially and temporally tailor their movements to aperture size.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The method of calculating the critical ratio used in the Wilmut and Barnett article was different from that described in the current article (curves were not fitted to the adult data). A different method was used with the children because many of them were showing larger shoulder rotations at the highest SA ratio used and so their critical ratio fell beyond the SA ratios measured, making curve fitting appropriate. Using the method described here on the adult data in the Wilmut and Barnett study produces a critical aperture of 1.45. This suggests that this method of determining the critical ratio may over-estimate it somewhat; however, it is still clear that the ratio seen in the child group is higher than that seen in the adult group.

  2. Time left after a reduction in speed was only calculated for trials where a reduction in speed occurred. A reduction in speed was seen 97% of 0.9 SA ratio trials, 89% of 1.1 SA ratio trials, 64% of 1.3 SA ratio trials, 64% of 1.5 SA ratio trials and 54% of 1.7 SA ratio trials.

References

  • Berard JR, Vallis LA (2006) Characteristics of single and double obstacle avoidance strategies: a comparison between adults and children. Exp Brain Res 175:21–31

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cesari P, Formenti F, Olivato P (2003) A common perceptual parameter for stair climbing for children, young and old adults. Hum Mov Sci 22:111–124

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cinelli ME, Patla AE (2008) Task-specific modulations of locomotor action parameters based on on-line visual information during collision avoidance with moving obstacles. Hum Mov Sci 27:513–531

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field A (2006) Discovering statistics using SPSS, 2nd edn. Sage, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Higuchi T, Cinelli ME, Greig MA, Patla AE (2006) Locomotion through apertures when wider space for locomotion is necessary: adaption to artificially altered body states. Exp Brain Res 175:50–59

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollands MA, Ziavra NV, Bronstein AM (2004) A new paradigm to investigate the roles of head and eye movement in the coordination of whole-body movements. Exp Brain Res 154:261–266

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McFadyen BJ, Malouin F, Dumas F (2001) Anticipatory locomotor control for obstacle avoidance in mid-childhood aged children. Gait Posture 13:7–16

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Montagne G, Buekers M, de Rugy A, Camachon C, Laurent M (2002) Control of human locomotion under various task constraints. Exp Brain Res 143:133–136

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pufall PB, Dunbar C (1992) Perceiving whether or not the world affords stepping onto and over: a developmental study. Ecol Psychol 4:17–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmuckler MA (1996) Development of visually guided locomotion: barrier crossing by toddlers. Ecol Psychol 8:209–236

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snapp-Childs W, Bingham GP (2009) The affordance of barrier crossing in young children exhibits dynamic, not geometric, similarity. Exp Brain Res 198:527–533

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich BD, Thelen E, Niles D (1990) Perceptual determinants of action: stair-climbing choices of infants and toddlers. In: Clark JE, Humphrey JH (eds) Advances in motor development research. AMS Press, New York, pp 1–15

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH (1984) Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:683–703

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH, Whang S (1987) Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:371–383

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilmut K, Barnett A (2010) Locomotor adjustments in adults when navigating through apertures. Hum Mov Sci 29:289–298

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Oxford Gait Laboratory at the Nuffield Orthopaedic Centre for the use of their VICON system and all of their help and advice, the School of Social Sciences and Law at Oxford Brookes University for funding this project and all of the children and families who took part.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kate Wilmut.

Appendix

Appendix

Safety margins were calculated by determining the relative distance between the shoulders as the participant moved through the door. The difference between this and the aperture size was taken to be the safety margin. Values were then normalised to shoulder width of the participant. Safety margins were only calculated for turn trials. The exclusion of no turn trials meant that safety margins were not calculated from a full data set. For adults, the 1.3 SA ratio safety margin was calculated for 7 out of 9 participants, and the 1.5 and 1.7 SA ratio could not be calculated. For children, safety margins for the 1.3 SA ratio included all children, the safety margin for the 1.5 SA ratio included 14 children and the safety margin for the 1.7 SA ratio included 4 children. Due to this loss of data, we will only compare safety margins for the 0.9, 1.1 and 1.3 SA ratio. It was not possible to combine the safety margins for the five SA ratios as variance was seen for both adults and children.

To investigate whether normalised safety margins were different across adults and children, we carried out independent samples t-tests (group) and found a significant effect of group for 0.9 SA ratio [t(23) = −2.336 P = 0.029] and an approaching significant effect for 1.1 SA ratio [t(23) = −1.815 P = 0.083]. The difference between adults and children suggests that even when shoulder width has been taken into account there is a difference between adults and children in terms of the safety margin left for the 0.9 and 1.1 SA ratio, this supports the Snapp-Childs and Bingham (2009) data. To further investigate the relationship between safety margins and movement variability, regression analyses were carried out between safety margins and the variability of safety margins. Neither the 0.9 nor the 1.1 SA ratio End wall showed a significant regression analysis.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wilmut, K., Barnett, A.L. Locomotor behaviour of children while navigating through apertures. Exp Brain Res 210, 185–194 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2614-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2614-2

Keywords

Navigation