Skip to main content
Log in

Signal detection theory and vestibular thresholds: I. Basic theory and practical considerations

  • Review
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 09 March 2012

Abstract

Detection theory has been applied to the measurement of vestibular thresholds and vestibular sensory integration. Yet, a formal detection theory analysis of vestibular responses has not been published. Such a de novo analysis seems warranted because the vestibular system has characteristics that differ from other sensory systems, which impacts the application of detection theory. For example, the physical stimuli evoking vestibular responses are typically bidirectional (e.g., leftward/rightward); this bidirectional nature of vestibular responses leads to another characteristic—what is sometimes called vestibular bias—that must also be considered, since it can impact threshold measurements, including thresholds found using staircase procedures. This paper develops a basic model of vestibular noise and then analyzes this model for four standard paradigms—one-interval recognition, one-interval detection, two-interval detection, and two-interval recognition. While any of these paradigms might be justified for a specific application, it is concluded that one-interval recognition paradigms have advantages over other paradigms for many vestibular applications. One-interval recognition is favored over one-interval detection because it lends itself to a fixed detection boundary, is more efficient, and is less impacted by device vibration. One-interval recognition is generally favored over two-interval recognition because it assesses vestibular bias and can require substantially less time than two-interval tasks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barnett-Cowan M, Harris LR (2009) Perceived timing of vestibular stimulation relative to touch, light and sound. Exp Brain Res 198:221–231

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benson AJ, Spencer MB, Stott JR (1986) Thresholds for the detection of the direction of whole-body, linear movement in the horizontal plane. Aviat Space Environ Med 57:1088–1096

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Benson A, Hutt E, Brown S (1989) Thresholds for the perception of whole body angular movement about a vertical axis. Aviat Space Environ Med 60:205–213

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell HR (1952) Studies of psychophysical methods for measuring visual thresholds. J Opt Soc Am 42:606–616

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brown RG, Hwang P (1992) Introduction to random signals and applied Kalman filtering. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter-Smith TR, Futamura RG, Parker DE (1995) Inertial acceleration as a measure of linear vection: an alternative to magnitude estimation. Percept Psychophys 57:35–42

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Clark B, Stewart JD (1968) Comparison of three methods to determine thresholds for perception of angular acceleration. Am J Psychol 81:207–216

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • De Vrijer M, Medendorp WP, Van Gisbergen JA (2008) Shared computational mechanism for tilt compensation accounts for biased verticality percepts in motion and pattern vision. J Neurophysiol 99:915–930

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doty RL (1969) Effect of duration of stimulus presentation on the angular acceleration threshold. J Exp Psychol 80:317–321

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gelb A (1974) Applied optimal estimation. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Grabherr L, Nicoucar K, Mast FW, Merfeld DM (2008) Vestibular thresholds for yaw rotation about an earth-vertical axis as a function of frequency. Exp Brain Res 186:677–681

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green D, Swets J (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gu Y, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE (2007) A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals. Nat Neurosci 10:1038–1047

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guedry F (1974) Psychophysics of vestibular sensation. In: Kornhuber HH (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology, vol VI. Springer-Verlag, New York, pp 1–154

    Google Scholar 

  • Hildebrand FB (1976) Advanced calculus for applications. Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs

    Google Scholar 

  • Jakel F, Wichmann FA (2006) Spatial four-alternative forced-choice method is the preferred psychophysical method for naive observers. J Vis 6:1307–1322

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones FN (1974) History of psychophysics and judgment. In: Carterette EC, Friedman MP (eds) Handbook of perception: vol. 2. Psychophysicla judgment and measurement. Academic Press, New York, pp 1–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay SM (1993) Fundamentals of statistical signal processing: estimation theory. Prentice Hall PTR, Upper Saddle River

    Google Scholar 

  • Kay SM (1998) Fundamentals of statistical signal processing: detection theory. Prentice Hall PTR, Upper Saddle River

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen RJ, Marx ML (1986) An introduction to mathematical statistics and its applications. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs

    Google Scholar 

  • Leek MR (2001) Adaptive procedures in psychophysical research. Percept Psychophys 63:1279–1292

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan NA, Creelman CD (2005) Detection theory: a user’s guide. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah

    Google Scholar 

  • MacNeilage PR, Banks MS, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE (2010) Vestibular heading discrimination and sensitivity to linear acceleration in head and world coordinates. J Neurosci 30:9084–9094

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mah RW, Young LR, Steele CR, Schubert ED (1989) Threshold perception of whole-body motion to linear sinusoidal stimulation. In: AlAA flight simulation technologies conference and exhibit, vol AIAA-89-3273, Boston, MA

  • Mallery RM, Olomu OU, Uchanski RM, Militchin VA, Hullar TE (2010) Human discrimination of rotational velocities. Exp Brain Res 204:11–20

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ormsby C (1974) Model of human dynamic orientation. In: Aeronautics and Astronautics, vol Ph.D. MIT, Cambridge

  • Pentland A (1980) Maximum likelihood estimation: the best PEST. Percept Psychophys 28:377–379

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sadeghi SG, Chacron MJ, Taylor MC, Cullen KE (2007) Neural variability, detection thresholds, and information transmission in the vestibular system. J Neurosci 27:771–781

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seidman SH (2008) Translational motion perception and vestiboocular responses in the absence of non-inertial cues. Exp Brain Res 184:13–29

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Treutwein B (1995) Adaptive psychophysical procedures. Vis Res 35:2503–2522

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Watson AB, Pelli DG (1983) QUEST: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys 33:113–120

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wichmann FA, Hill NJ (2001a) The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Percept Psychophys 63:1293–1313

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wichmann FA, Hill NJ (2001b) The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling. Percept Psychophys 63:1314–1329

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren WA (1968) Unidimensional strength theory and component analysis of noise in absolute and comparative judgments. J Math Psychol 5:102–122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wikipedia (2010a) Central limit theorem. http://www.en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Central_limit_theorem

  • Wikipedia (2010b) Error function. http://www.en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Error_function

  • Zupan LH, Merfeld DM (2008) Interaural self-motion linear velocity thresholds are shifted by roll vection. Exp Brain Res 191:505–511

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Funding for this experiment was provided in part by the NIH (NIDCD R01 04158). We would also like to thank Margaret Lankow for administrative assistance, Csilla Haburcakova and Saori Fukuda for help with the graphics, and Shomesh Chaudhuri, Michael Barnett-Cowan, Luzia Grabherr, Tim Hullar, Faisal Karmali, Koeun Lim, Fred Mast, Chuck Oman, and Florian Soyka, who provided comments on preliminary drafts. Thanks also to Jay Goldberg, who has inspired the use of such theoretical analyses in the vestibular sciences alongside thorough experimental evaluations of such analyses.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel M. Merfeld.

Additional information

An erratum to this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3024-9.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Merfeld, D.M. Signal detection theory and vestibular thresholds: I. Basic theory and practical considerations. Exp Brain Res 210, 389–405 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2557-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2557-7

Keywords

Navigation