Skip to main content
Log in

Doing better than your best: loud auditory stimulation yields improvements in maximal voluntary force

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Could task performance be constrained by our ability to fully engage necessary neural processing through effort of will? The StartReact phenomenon suggests that this might be the case, as voluntary reaction times are substantially reduced by loud sounds. Here, we show that loud auditory stimulation can also be associated with an improvement in the force and speed of force development when 18 healthy subjects are repeatedly asked to make a maximal grip as fast and as strongly as possible. Peak grip force was increased by 7.2 ± 1.4% (SEM) (P < 0.0001), and the rate of force development was increased by 17.6 ± 2.0% (P ≪ 0.00001), when imperative visual cues were accompanied by a loud auditory stimulus rather than delivered alone. This implies that loud auditory stimuli may allow motor pathways to be optimised beyond what can be achieved by effort of will alone.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Angel A (1973) Input-output relations in simple reaction time experiments. Quart J Exp Psych 25:193–200

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner T, Willi M, Jäncke L (2007) Modulation of corticospinal activity by strong emotions evoked by pictures and classical music: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport 18:261–265

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bohannon RW, Peolsson A, Massy-Westropp N, Desrosiers J, Bear-Lehman J (2006) Reference values for adult grip strength measured with a Jamar dynamometer: a descriptive meta-analysis. Physiotherapy 92:11–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonanni L, Thomas A, Onofrj M (2010) Paradoxical kinesia in patients surviving earthquake. Mov Disorder 25:1302–1304

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown P, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Britton TC, Day BL, Marsden CD (1991) New observations on the normal auditory startle reflex in man. Brain 114:1891–1902

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsen AN, Chua R, Inglis JT, Sanderson DJ, Franks IM (2004) Prepared movements are elicited early by startle. J Mot Behav 36:253–264

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlsen AN, Chua R, Inglis T, Sanderson DJ, Franks IM (2007) Differential effects of startle on reaction time for finger and arm movements. J Neurophysiol 101:306–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coombes SA, Cauraugh JH, Janelle CM (2007) Emotional state and initiating cue alter central and peripheral motor processes. Emotion 7:275–284

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coombes SA, Tandonnet C, Fujiyama H, Janelle CM, Cauraugh JH, Summers JJ (2009) Emotion and motor preparation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study of corticospinal motor tract excitability. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 9:380–388

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daroff RB (2008) Paradoxical Kinesia. Mov Disord 23:1193

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dufft CC, Ulrich R (1999) Intersensory facilitation: also concomitant visual stimuli can decrease reaction time. Z Exp Psychol 46:16–27

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fess EE (1992) Grip strength. In: Casanova JS (ed) Clinical assessment recommendations. American Society of Hand therapists, Chicago, pp 41–45

    Google Scholar 

  • Giray M, Ulrich R (1993) Motor coactivation revealed by response force in divided and focused attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perf 19:1278–1291

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glickstein M, Stein J (1991) Paradoxical movement in Parkinson’s disease. Tr Neurosci 14:480–482

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Househam E, McAuley J, Charles T, Lightfoot T, Swash M (2004) Analysis of force profile during a maximum voluntary isometric contraction task. Muscle Nerve 29:401–408

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jaśkowski P, Rybarczyk K, Jaroszyk F, Lemański D (1995) The effect of stimulus intensity on force output in simple reaction time task in humans. Acta Neurobiol Exp 55:57–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Jepma M, Wagenmakers EJ, Band GP, Nieuwenhuis S (2009) The effects of accessory stimuli on information processing: evidence from electrophysiology and a diffusion model analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 21:847–864

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langerström C, Nordgren B (1998) On the reliability and usefulness of methods for grip strength measurement. Scand J Rehabil Med 30:113–119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liegeois-Chauvel C, Morin C, Musolino A, Bancaud J, Chauvel P (1989) Evidence for a contribution of the auditory cortex to audiospinal facilitation in man. Brain 112:375–391

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni P, Hristova A, Krakauer JW (2007) Why don’t we move faster? Parkinson’s disease, movement vigor and implicit motivation. J Neurosci 27:7105–7116

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller J, Franz V, Ulrich R (1999) Effects of auditory stimulus intensity on response force in simple, go/no-go, and choice RT tasks. Percept Psychophys 61:107–119

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mordkoff JT, Miller JO, Roch AC (1996) Absence of coactivation in the motor component: evidence from psychophysiological measures of target detection. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perf 22:25–41

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Nissen MJ, Klein R (1976) Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychol Rev 83:157–171

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Queralt A, Weerdesteyn V, van Duijnhoven HJ, Castellote JM, Valls-Solé J, Duysens J (2008) The effects of an auditory startle on obstacle avoidance during walking. J Physiol 586:4453–4463

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds RF, Day BL (2007) Fast visuomotor processing made faster by sound. J Physiol 583:1107–1115

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders AF (1983) Towards a model of stress and human performance. Acta Psychol 53:61–97

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt L, Cléry-Melin ML, Lafargue G, Valabrègue R, Fossati P, Dubois B, Pessiglione M (2009) Get aroused and be stronger: emotional facilitation of physical effort in the human brain. J Neurosci 29:9450–9457

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl J, Rammsayer TH (2005) Accessory stimulation in the time course of visuomotor information processing: stimulus intensity effects on reaction time and response force. Acta Psychol 120:1–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valls-Solé J, Rothwell JC, Goulart F, Cossu G, Muñoz JE (1999) Patterned ballistic movements triggered by a startle in healthy humans. J Physiol 516:931–938

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth RS (1938) Reaction time. In: Experimental psychology, Holt, New York, pp 317–326

Download references

Acknowledgments

PB is supported by the Medical Research Council and the Oxford NIHR Biomedical Centre.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Brown.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Anzak, A., Tan, H., Pogosyan, A. et al. Doing better than your best: loud auditory stimulation yields improvements in maximal voluntary force. Exp Brain Res 208, 237–243 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2474-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2474-1

Keywords

Navigation