Skip to main content
Log in

The use of predictive information is impaired in the actions of children and young adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The need for a movement response may often be preceded by some advance information regarding direction or extent. We examined the ability of individuals with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) to organise a movement in response to advance information. Pre-cues were presented and varied in the extent to which they indicated the response target. Both eye movement latencies and hand movements were measured. In the absence of pre-cues, individuals with DCD were as fast in initial hand movements as the typically developing (TD) participants, but were less efficient at correcting initial directional errors. A major difference was seen in the degree to which each group could use advance pre-cue information. TD participants were able to use pre-cue information to refine their actions. For the individuals with DCD this was only effective if there was no ambiguity in the advance cue and they had particular difficulty in using predictive motion cues. There were no differences in the speed of gaze responses which excluded an explanation relating to the dynamic allocation of attention. Individuals with DCD continued to rely on the slower strategy of fixating the target prior to initiating a hand movement, rather than using advance information to set initial movement parameters.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The saccade onset latencies are quite long for all children, although the standard deviations are relatively small. It should be noted that this was not a simple saccadic task, but required saccades to an unpredictable 4-choice peripheral array. Rolfs and Vitu (2007) used similar target arrays for a saccadic task with adults and reported mean saccade latencies of up to 340 ms with but some trial latencies above 400 ms. The latencies we report are consistent with this and what we have observed in children on other tasks (Wilmut et al. 2006).

  2. For the static cueing condition the lack of an advantage to a partial cue in children with DCD has been attributed to the high cost of generating a movement to an incorrect target location (Mon-Williams et al. 2005). An elongated ISI would not serve to offset this. If the problem in using predictive motion is processing and extrapolating motion direction then the length of the ISI may well be the cogent variable.

  3. None of the participants with DCD fell outside the confidence intervals of the TD group (confidence intervals were calculated separately for the primary and secondary age group due to the age difference seen in thresholds).

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edn edn. APA, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Annett M (1970) The growth of manual performance and speed. Br J Psychol 61:545–558

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bairstow PJ (1987) Analysis of hand movement to moving targets. Hum Mov Sci 6:205–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bairstow PJ (1989) Development of planning and control of hand movements to moving target. Br J Dev Psychol 7:29–42

    Google Scholar 

  • Bairstow PJ, Laszlo JI (1989) Deficits in the planning, control and recall of hand movements in children with perceptuo-motor dysfunction. Br J Dev Psychol 7:251–273

    Google Scholar 

  • Braddick OJ, O’Brien JMD, Wattam-Bell J, Atkinson J, Turner R (2000) Form and motion coherence activate independent, but not dorsal/ventral segregated, networks in the human brain. Curr Biol 10:731–741

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112(1):155–159

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Estil LB, Ingvaldsen RP, Whiting HTA (2002) Spatial and temporal constraints on performance in children with movement coordination problems. Exp Brain Res 147:153–161

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Field A (2006) Discovering statistics using SPSS, 2nd edn edn. Sage, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Gordon N, McKinlay S (1980) Helping clumsy children. Churchill Livingstone, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunn A, Cory E, Atkinson J, Braddick O, Wattam-Bell J, Guzzetta A et al (2002) Dorsal and ventral stream sensitivity in normal development and hemiplegia. NeuroReport 13(6):843–847

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson SE, Sugden DA (1992) Movement assessment battery for children. The psychological corporation, Sidcup

  • Losse A, Henderson S, Elliman D, Hall D, Knight E, Jongmans M (1991) Clumsiness in children—do they grow out of it? A 10 year follow up study. Dev Med Child Neurol 33:55–68

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mandich A, Buckolz E, Polatajko H (2003) Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and their ability to disengage ongoing attentional focus: more on inhibitory function. Brain Cogn 51(3):346–356

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mon-Williams MA, Tresilian JR, Bell VE, Coppard VL, Nixdorf M, Carson RG (2005) The preparation of reach-to-grasp movements in adults, children, and children with movement problems. Q J Exp Psychol 58A(7):1249–1263

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien J, Spencer J, Atkinson J, Braddick O, Wattam-Bell J (2002) Form and motion coherence processing in dyspraxia: evidence of a global spatial processing deficit. Cogn Neurosci 13(11):1399–1402

    Google Scholar 

  • Olivier I, Audiffren M, Ripoll H (1998) Age-related differences in preparatory processes of motor programming. J Exp Child Psychol 69(1):49–65

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Olivier I, Bard C (2000) The effects of spatial movement components pre-cues on the execution of rapid aiming in children aged 7, 9, and 11. J Exp Child Psychol 77(2):155–168

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Riggo L, Dascola I, Umilta C (1987) Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favour of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 25:31–40

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rolfs M, Vitu F (2007) On the limited role of target onset in the gap task: Support for the motor-preparation hypothesis. J Vis 7(10):1–20

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum DA (1980) Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction and extent. J Exp Psychol Gen 109:444–474

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scase MO, Braddick OJ, Raymond JE (1996) What is noise for the motion system? Vis Res 36:2579–2589

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schellekens JHM, Huizing F, Kalverboer AF (1986) The influence of movement amplitude on precue-processing. Hum Mov Sci 5:249–262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schellekens JHM, Kalverboer AF, Scholten CS (1984) The microstructure of tapping movements in children. J Mot Behav 16:20–39

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schulman JL, Buist C, Kaspar JC, Child D, Fackler E (1969) An objective test of speed of fine motor function. Percept Mot Skills 29:243–255

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sheliga BM, Craighero L, Riggio G, Rizzolatti G (1997) Effects of spatial attention on directional manual and ocular responses. Exp Brain Res 114:339–351

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman GL, Ollinger JM, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Peterson SE et al (1999) Areas involved in encoding and applying directional expectations to moving objects. J Neurosci 19(21):9480–9496

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sigmundsson H, Hansenc PC, Talcott JB (2003) Do “clumsy” children have visual deficits. Behav Brain Res 139(1–2):123–129

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stein J, Walsh V (1997) To see but not to read: the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci 20:147–152

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sugden DA, Sugden LG (1992) The assessment of movement skill problems in 7- and 9-year-old children. Br J Dev Psychol 61:329–345

    Google Scholar 

  • Sugden DA, Wright HC (1998) Motor coordination disorders in children, vol 39. SAGE, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dellen T, Geuze RH (1988) Motor response processing in clumsy children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 29(4):489–500

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dellen T, Geuze RH (1990) Development of auditory precue processing during a movement sequence. J Hum Mov Stud 18:229–241

    Google Scholar 

  • Visser J (2003) Developmental coordination disorder: a review of research on subtypes and comorbidities. Hum Mov Sci 22:479–493

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilmut K, Brown JH, Wann JP (2007) Attention disengagement in children with developmental coordination disorder. Disabil Rehabil 29(1):47–55

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilmut K, Wann JP, Brown JH (2006) Problems in the coupling of eye and hand in the sequential movements of children with developmental coordination disorder. Child Care Health Dev 32(6):665–678

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson PH, Maruff P, McKenzie BE (1997) Covert orienting of visuospatial attention in children with developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol 39(11):736–745

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Visual Developmental Unit at Oxford University for supplying us with the form and motion coherence tests. We would also like to thank the Dyspraxia Foundation and all of our participants, without whom this research could not have taken place. This report was prepared with support from the Economic and Social Research Council (RES-000-22-1266).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kate Wilmut.

Appendix I

Appendix I

Additional analyses were carried out on the components of the hand movement measure (hand onset, average velocity and heading error at 200 ms) for percentage improved values of these see Table 3.

For hand onset a main effect of cue and group was found for, static peripheral cueing [cue, F(1,42) = 16.97 P < 0.001 η2 = 0.22, group F(1,42) = 4.64 P = 0.037 η2 = 0.10], static central cueing [cue, F(1,42) = 13.34 P = 0.001 η2 = 0.24, group, F(1,42) = 13.75 P = 0.001 η2 = 0.25], predictive motion 4 target cueing [cue, F(2,84) = 20.69 P < 0.001 η2 = 0.33, group, F(142) = 15.12 P < 0.001 η2 = 0.26] and predictive motion 12 target cueing [cue, F(2,84) = 12.71 P < 0.001 η2 = 0.23, group, F(1,42) = 18.18 P < 0.001 η2 = 0.30].

For average velocity a main effect of cue was found for, static peripheral cueing [F(1,42) = 36.42 P < 0.001 η2 = 0.46], static central cueing [F(1,42) = 6.28 P = 0.016 η2 = 0.130], predictive motion 4 target cueing [F(2,84) = 2.53 P = 0.086 η2 = 0.057] and predictive motion 12 target cueing [F(2,84) = 1.69 P = 0.191]. No effects of groups were found.

For heading error 200 ms after hand onset the results were as follows: static peripheral cueing [group, F(1,42) = 10.18 P = 0.003 η2 = 0.19, no effect of cue]; static central cueing [cue, F(1,42) = 34.49 P < 0.001 η2 = 0.45, group, F(1,42) = 11.70 P = 0.001 η2 = 0.22]; predictive motion 4 target cueing [group, F(1,42) = 10.67 P = 0.002 η2 = 0.20, no effect of cue]; and predictive motion 12 target cueing [cue, F(2,84) = 6.24 P = 0.003 η2 = 0.13, group, F(1,42) = 7.41 P = 0.009 η2 = 0.15].

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wilmut, K., Wann, J. The use of predictive information is impaired in the actions of children and young adults with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Exp Brain Res 191, 403–418 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1532-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1532-4

Keywords

Navigation