Skip to main content
Log in

Perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface depends on height of center of mass

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated whether perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface depended on the height of center of mass of the perceiver-actor. Participants adjusted the angle of inclination of a surface until they felt that it was just barely possible for them to stand on that surface. They performed this task while wearing a backpack apparatus to which masses were attached in one of three configurations—high-mass, low-mass, and no-mass. Moreover, participants performed this task by viewing the inclined surface or by probing it with a hand-held rod (while blindfolded). Perception of affordances for standing on the inclined surface reflected the changes in center of mass brought on by the weighted backpack apparatus (the perceptual boundary occurred at a smaller angle of inclination in the high-mass condition than in the low-mass condition and in the no-mass condition). Moreover, perception of this affordance reflected such changes both when the surface was viewed and when the surface was probed with a hand-held rod (while blindfolded). The results highlight that perception of affordances is dynamic and task-dependent and suggest that the stimulation patterns that support perception of affordances are invariant and modality-independent.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Anectodally, we can report that exploration of the inclined surface by means of remote haptic perception was quite variable and highly individualized. Participants explored the surface in a number of ways (tapping, rubbing, probing, scraping, poking) in a number of directions (side to side, up and down) using a variety of joint movements (wrist, elbow, shoulder). Although we did not record exploration time, we can anecdotally report that (a) exploration time varied, but for the most part, bouts of exploration were relatively short (just as few seconds), (b) participants tended to explore for longer periods of time when probing slopes near the perceptual boundary, and (c) participants tended to explore for longer periods of time when probing the surface than when viewing the surface (Fitzpatrick et al. 1994).

  2. The variability of perceptual responses across the four trials in a given condition was quite small. Across participants and conditions, the standard deviation of the four perceptual reports ranged from 2.3° to 9.6°. On average the standard deviation of the four trials in a given condition was 3.5°.

  3. The lack of difference between perceptual and behavioral boundaries (when participants were not wearing a backpack) may seem inconsistent with Bhalla and Proffitt’s (1999) finding that participants overestimate the inclination of hills (regardless of whether they are wearing a weighted backpack). However, it is important to point out that such overestimations were (verbal) estimates of the angle of inclination, not (yes or no) reports of whether the surface afforded standing on (as in the current experiment).

References

  • Adolph KE (1997) Learning in the development of infant locomotion. Monographs for the society for research in child development, vol 62 (3, Serial No. 251)

  • Adolph KE, Avolio AM (2000) Walking infants adapt locomotion to changing body dimensions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:1148–1166

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bhalla M, Proffitt DR (1999) Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 25:1076–1096

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP (1988) Task-specific devices and the perceptual bottleneck. Hum Mov Sci 7:225–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham GP, Schmidt RC, Rosenblum LD (1989) Hefting for a maximum distance throw: A smart perceptual mechanism. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 15:507–528

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bongers RM, Michaels CF, Smitsman AW (2004) Variations of tool and task characteristics reveal that tool-use postures are anticipated. J Mot Behav 36:305–315

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burton G (1993) Non-neural extensions of haptic sensitivity. Ecol Psychol 5:105–124

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carello C, Fitzpatrick P, Domaniewicz I, Chan TC, Turvey MT (1992) Effortful touch with minimal movement. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:290–302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carello C, Wagman JB, Turvey MT (2005) Acoustic specification of object properties. In: Anderson J, Anderson B (eds) Moving image theory: ecological considerations. Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, pp 79–104

    Google Scholar 

  • Chemero A (2003) An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecol Psychol 15:181–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzpatrick P, Carello C, Schmidt RC, Corey D (1994) Haptic and visual perception of an affordance for upright posture. Ecol Psychol 6:265–287

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson EJ (1969) Principles of perceptual learning and development. Appleton Century-Crofts, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson EJ, Pick AD (2000) An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Higuchi T, Takada H, Matsuura Y, Imanaka K (2004) Visual estimation of spatial requirements for locomotion in novice wheelchair users. J Exp Psychol Appl 10:55–66

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirose N, Nishio A (2001) The process of adaptation to perceiving new action capabilities. Ecol Psychol 13:49–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joh AS, Adolph KE, Narayanan PJ, Dietz VA (2007) Gauging possibilities for action based on friction underfoot. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33:1145–1157

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsella-Shaw JM, Shaw B, Turvey MT (1992) Perceiving “walk-on-able” slopes. Ecol Psychol 4:223–239

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konczak J, Meeuwsen HJ, Cress ME (1992) Changing affordances in stair climbing: the perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:691–697

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Malek EA, Wagman JB (2008) Kinetic potential influences visual and remote haptic perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface. Q J Exp Psychol (in press)

  • Mark LS (1987) Eyeheight scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:361–370

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mark LS, Balliet JA, Craver KD, Douglas SD, Fox T (1990) What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. Ecol Psychol 2:325–366

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans RR, Michaels CF, Bakker FC, Dolne MA (1996) The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: perception of catchableness of fly balls. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:879–891

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pijpers JR, Oudejans RRD, Bakker FC (2007) Changes in the perception of action possibilities while climbing to fatigue on a climbing wall. J Sports Sci 25:97–110

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson MJ, Marsh KL, Baron R (2007) Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 33:845–859

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblum LD, Gordon MS (2001) The generality of specificity: Some lessons from audiovisual speech. Behav Brain Sci 24:239–240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runeson S (1977) On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms. Scand J Psychol 18:172–179

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Bardy B (2001) On specification and the senses. Behav Brain Sci 24:195–261

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tuller B, Case P, Ding MZ, Kelso JAS (1994) The nonlinear dynamics of speech categorization. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 20:3–16

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT (1992) Affordances and prospective control: an outline of the ontology. Ecol Psychol 4:173–187

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT (2004) Space and its perception: the first and final frontier. Ecol Psychol 16:25–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turvey MT, Shaw RE (1999) Ecological foundations of cognition: I. symmetry and specificity of animal-environment systems. J Conscious Stud 6:95–110

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagman JB, Carello C (2001) Affordances and inertial constraints on tool use. Ecol Psychol 13:173–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman JB, Malek EA (2007) Perception of whether an object can be carried through an aperture depends on anticipated speed. Exp Psychol 54:54–61

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagman JB, Taylor KR (2005) Perceiving affordances for aperture crossing for the person-plus-object system. Ecol Psychol 17:105–130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH (1984) Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10:683–703

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by an Illinois State University Summer Faculty Fellowship awarded to J.B.W. We thank Eric Malek for his help with data collection and Dawn McBride for helpful discussion. We thank three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on a previous draft of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jeffrey B. Wagman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Regia-Corte, T., Wagman, J.B. Perception of affordances for standing on an inclined surface depends on height of center of mass. Exp Brain Res 191, 25–35 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1492-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1492-8

Keywords

Navigation