Skip to main content
Log in

Collision avoidance behavior as a function of aging and tennis playing

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Daily living often requires pedestrians and drivers to adapt their behavior to the displacement of other objects in their environment in order to avoid collision. Yet little research has paid attention to the effect of age on the completion of such a challenging task. The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between age and collision avoidance skill and whether a sporting activity affects this. Three age groups (20–30, 60–70, and 70–80 years) of tennis players and non-players launched a projectile toward a target in order to hit it before it was hit by another “object” (a stimulus represented by apparent motion of lights). If the participant judged that time-to-collision (TTC) of the moving stimulus was not long enough for him/her to launch the projectile in time to arrive before the stimulus, the participant had to inhibit the launching. Results showed that for the non-players the number of errors in the 70–80 year-old group was significantly higher than those of the 20–30 and 60–70 year-old groups, which did not differ from each other. However, this increase was not observed in the 70-80 year-old tennis players, demonstrating a beneficial effect of playing tennis on collision avoidance skill. Results also revealed that the older groups of both tennis players and non-players were subject to the typical age-related increase in response time. Additional analyses indicated that the 70–80 year-old non-players did not adjust their actions to these age-related changes in response time. The older tennis-playing participants, however, were more likely to adjust collision avoidance behavior to their diminished response times.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Standard deviations (SD) of TT, DT and LT were also calculated and submitted to an age3 × tennis playing2 ANOVA. The analysis on SD TT yielded no significant effect or interaction. The analysis on SD DT yielded a main effect of age, F(2,42) = 4.31, P < 0.05, η 2 = 0.17, and tennis playing, F(1,42) = 10.18, P < 0.01, η 2 = 0.20, and an interaction between these two factors, F(2,42) = 3.91, P < 0.05, η 2 = 0.16. A post hoc test on this interaction yielded significant differences between the 70–80 year-old non-player group and the other groups. The analysis on SD LT yielded a main effect of age, F(2,42) = 8.71, P < 0.001, η 2 = 0.29, due to a more variable LT for the 70–80 year-olds than for the other two groups, as well as a main effect of tennis playing, F(1,42) = 9.83, P < 0.01, η 2 = 0.19, the non-players having a more variable LT than the players. SDs are given for each group in Table 1.

  2. To examine participants’ abilities to adjust behavior to the available time, the relationships between response times (TT, DT and LT) and TTC were assessed. To this end, the individual linear relationships of individual TT, DT and LT as a function of TTC were calculated for each participant separately. Fisher Z-transformations of the correlation coefficients (r), slopes, and intercepts of the individual linear regressions were analysed. Analyses of the TT/TTC relationships revealed age differences on Fisher Z-scores, F(2,42) = 4.87, P < 0.05, η 2 = 0.19, which were higher for the young group than for the older groups, and on the intercept, F(2,42) = 9.04, P < 0.001, η 2 = 0.30; which was lower for the young group than for the older groups. Analyses of the DT/TTC relationships did not reveal significant effects or interactions. Analyses of the LT/TTC relationships yielded age differences on the Z-scores, F(2,42) = 3.52, P < 0.05, η 2 = 0.14, which were higher for the young group than for the older groups, and on the intercept, F(2,42) = 5.31, P < 0.01, η 2 = 0.20, which was higher for the 70–80 year-olds than for the both other groups. The low slopes and Z scores of the relationship between TTC and response times, and the lack of differences between ages or tennis-playing groups suggests that the behavioral adjustment to TTC was minimal, indicating that the participants, whatever their age and tennis-playing group, were likely to use a constant response-time strategy to perform the task (Table 1).

References

  • Andersen GJ, Enriquez AJ (2006) Aging and the detection of observer and moving object collisions. Psychol Aging 21(1):74–85

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baylor AM, Spirduso WW (1988) Systematic aerobic exercise and components of reaction time in older women. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 43:121–126

    Google Scholar 

  • Bosman EA (1993) Age-related differences in motoric aspects of transcription typing skill. Psychol Aging 8(1):87–102

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bosman EA (1994) Age and skill differences in typing related and unrelated reaction time tasks. Aging Cogn 1:310–322

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavallo V, Lobjois R, Vienne F (2006) The interest of an interactive road crossing simulation for the study of adaptive road crossing behavior. In: 1st driving simulation conference Asia-Pacific, Tsukuba, Japan

  • Charness N, Bosman EA (1990) Expertise and aging: Life in the lab. In: Hess TM (eds) Aging and cognition: knowledge organization and utilization, Elsevier Science Publishers, North Holland, pp 343–386

  • Christensen CL, Payne VG, Wughalter EH, Yan JH, Henehan M, Jones R (2003) Physical activity, physiological, and psychomotor performance: a study of variously active older adult men. Res Q Exerc Sport 74(2):136–142

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Churchill JD, Galvez R, Colcombe S, Swain RA, Kramer AF, Greenough WT (2002) Exercise, experience and the aging brain. Neurobiol Aging 23(5):941–955

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarkson-Smith L, Hartley AA (1990) The game of bridge as an exercise in working memory and reasoning. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 45:233–238

    Google Scholar 

  • Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF (2003) Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci 14(2):125–130

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Raz N, Webb AG, Cohen NJ, McAuley E, Kramer AF (2003) Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 58:176–180

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Del Rey P (1982) Effects of contextual interference on the memory of older females differing in levels of physical activity. Percept Mot Skills 55:171–180

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeLucia PR, Bleckley MK, Meyer LE, Bush JM (2003) Judgments about collision in younger and older drivers. Transp Res Part F 6(1):63–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Dustman RE, Emmerson R, Shearer D (1994) Physical activity, age, and cognitive-neuropsychological function. J Aging Phys Act 2(2):143–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Emery CF, Huppert FA, Schein RL (1995) Relationships among age, exercise, health, and cognitive function in a British sample. Gerontologist 35:378–385

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Etnier JL, Salazar W, Landers DM, Petruzzello SJ, Han M, Nowell P (1997) The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. J Sport Exerc Psychol 19:249–277

    Google Scholar 

  • Etnier JL, Sibley BA, Pomeroy J, Kao JC (2003) Components of response time as a function of age, physical activity, and aerobic fitness. J Aging Phys Act 11(3):319–332

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisk AD, Rogers WA (2000) Influence of training and experience on skill acquisition and maintenance in older adults. J Aging Phys Act 8(4):373–378

    Google Scholar 

  • Gérin-Lajoie M, Richards CL, McFayden BJ (2006) The circumvention of obstacles during walking in different environmental contexts: a comparison between older and younger adults. Gait Posture 24(3):364–369

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1979) An ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci 15(1):20–25

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hancock PA, Manser MP (1997) Time-to-contact: more than tau alone. Ecol Psychol 9(4):265–297

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrell WA (1991) Precautionary street crossing by elderly pedestrians. Int J Aging Hum Dev 32(1):65–80

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Harruff RC, Avery A, Alter-Pandya AS (1998) Analysis of circumstances and injuries in 217 pedestrian traffic fatalities. Accid Anal Prev 30(1):11–20

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haywood KM (1980) Coincidence-anticipation accuracy across the life span. Exp Aging Res 6:451–462

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Holland C, Rabbitt P (1992) People’s awareness of their age-related sensory and cognitive deficits and the implications for road safety. Appl Cogn Psychol 6(3):217–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovacs CR (2005) Age-related changes in gait and obstacle avoidance capabilities in older adults: a review. J Appl Gerontol 24(1):21–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer AF, Colcombe S, Erickson K, Belopolsky A, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, Webb A, Jerome GJ, Marquez DX, Wszalek TM (2002) Effects of aerobic fitness training on human cortical function: a proposal. J Mol Neurosci 19(1–2):227–231

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer AF, Behrer L, Colcombe SJ, Dong W, Greenough WT (2004) Environmental influences on cognitive and brain plasticity during aging. J Gerontol Med Sci 59(9):940–957

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Young DS, McLaughlin CM (1984) A roadside simulation of road crossing for children. Ergonomics 27(12):1271–1281

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lobjois R, Cavallo V (2007) Age-related differences in street-crossing decisions: the effects of vehicle speed and time constraints on gap selection in an estimation task. Accid Anal Prev (in press). doi:10.10 16/j.aap.20 06.12.013

  • Lobjois R, Benguigui N, Bertsch J (2006) The effect of aging and tennis playing on coincidence-timing accuracy. J Aging Phys Act 14(1):74–97

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lupinacci NS, Rikli RE, Jones CJ, Ross D (1993) Age and physical activity effects on reaction time and digit symbol substitution performance in cognitively active adults. Res Q Exerc Sport 64:144–150

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Milner AD, Goodale MA (1995) The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans RRD, Michaels CF, van Dort B, Frissen EJP (1996) To cross or not to cross: the effect of locomotion on street-crossing behavior. Ecol Psychol 8(3):259–267

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oxley J, Fildes B, Ihsen E, Charlton J, Day R (2005) Crossing roads safely: an experimental study of age differences in gap selection by pedestrians. Accid Anal Prev 37(5):962–971

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plumert JM, Kearney JK, Cremer JF (2004) Children’s perception of gap affordances: bicycling across traffic-filled intersections in an immersive virtual environment. Child Dev 75(4):1243–1253

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rikli RE, Edwards DJ (1991) Effects of a three-year exercise program on motor function and cognitive processing speed in older women. Res Q Exerc Sport 62:61–67

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Salthouse TA (1984) Effects of age and skill in typing. J Exp Psychol Gen 113:345–371

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff W, Detwiler ML (1979) Information used in judging impending collision. Perception 8(6):647–658

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff W, Oldak R (1990) Accuracy of judging time to arrival: effects of modality, trajectory and gender. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16(2):303–316

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shay KA, Roth DL (1992) Association between aerobic fitness and visuospatial performance in healthy older adults. Psychol Aging 7:15–24

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simpson G, Johnston L, Richardson M (2003) An investigation of road crossing in a virtual environment. Accid Anal Prev 35(5):787–796

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spirduso WW (1975) Reaction and movement time as a function of age and physical activity level. J Gerontol 30:435–440

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spirduso WW, Clifford P (1978) Replications of age and physical activity effects on reaction and movement time. J Gerontol 33:26–30

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Te Velde AF, Van Der Kamp J, Barela JA, Savelsbergh GJP (2005) Visual timing and adaptative behavior in a road-crossing simulation study. Accid Anal Prev 37(3):399–406

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tresilian JR (1995) Perceptual and cognitive processes in time-to-contact estimation: analysis of prediction-motion and relative judgments tasks. Percept Psychophys 57(2):231–245

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Warren WH (1984) Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 10(5):683–703

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the French Ministry for Research and Education to Régis Lobjois while he was with the Center for Research in Sport Sciences, Faculty of Sport Sciences, University of Paris-Sud, Orsay, France.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Régis Lobjois.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lobjois, R., Benguigui, N., Bertsch, J. et al. Collision avoidance behavior as a function of aging and tennis playing. Exp Brain Res 184, 457–468 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1117-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1117-7

Keywords

Navigation