Skip to main content
Log in

Development of multisensory reweighting for posture control in children

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Reweighting to multisensory inputs adaptively contributes to stable and flexible upright stance control. However, few studies have examined how early a child develops multisensory reweighting ability, or how this ability develops through childhood. The purpose of the study was to characterize a developmental landscape of multisensory reweighting for upright postural control in children 4–10 years of age. Children were presented with simultaneous small-amplitude somatosensory and visual environmental movement at 0.28 and 0.2 Hz, respectively, within five conditions that independently varied the amplitude of the stimuli. The primary measure was body sway amplitude relative to each stimulus: touch gain and vision gain. We found that children can reweight to multisensory inputs from 4 years on. Specifically, intra-modal reweighting was exhibited by children as young as 4 years of age; however, inter-modal reweighting was only observed in the older children. The amount of reweighting increased with age indicating development of a better adaptive ability. Our results rigorously demonstrate the development of simultaneous reweighting to two sensory inputs for postural control in children. The present results provide further evidence that the development of multisensory reweighting contributes to more stable and flexible control of upright stance, which ultimately serves as the foundation for functional behaviors such as locomotion and reaching.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Reference

  • Allison LK, Kiemel T, Jeka JJ (2006) Multisensory reweighting of vision and touch is intact in healthy and fall-prone older adults. Exp Brain Res 175:342–352

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barela JA, Jeka JJ, Clark JE (1999) The use of somatosensory information during the acquisition of independent upright stance. Infant Behav Dev 22:87–102

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barela JA, Jeka JJ, Clark JE (2003) Postural control in children—coupling to dynamic somatosensory information. Exp Brain Res 150:434–442

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bendat JS, Piersol AG (2000) Random data: analyis and measurement procedures. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertenthal BI, Bai DL (1989) Infants’ sensitivity to optical flow for controlling posture. Dev Psychol 25:936–945

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertenthal BI, Bai DL (1997) Perception-action coupling in the development of visual control of posture. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:1631–1643

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carver S, Kiemel T, van der Kooij H, Jeka JJ (2005) Comparing internal models of the dynamics of the visual environment. Biol Cybern 92:147–163

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429–433

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher NI (1995) Statistical analysis of circular data. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Forssberg H, Nashner LM (1982) Ontogenetic development of postural control in man: adaptation to altered support and visual conditions during stance. J Neurosci 2:545–552

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Foster EC, Sveistrup H, Woollacott MH (1996) Transitions in visual proprioception: a cross-sectional developmental study of the effect of visual flow on postural control. J Motor Behav 28:101–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foudriat BA, Di Fabio RP, Anderson JH (1993) Sensory organization of balance responses in children 3–6 years of age: a normative study with diagnostic implications. Int J Pediatric Otorhinolaryngol 27:255–271

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glad T, Ljung L (2000) Control theory: multivariable and nonlinear methods. Taylor & Francis, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay JC, Pick HL, Ikeda K (1965) Visual capture produced by prism spectacles. Psychon Sci 2:215–216

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson SE, Sugden D (1992) Movement Assessment Battery for Children. The Psychological Corporation, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Hochberg Y, Tamhane AC (1987) Multiple comparison procedures. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeka J, Oie KS, Kiemel T (2000) Multisensory information for human postural control: integrating touch and vision. Exp Brain Res 134:107–125

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jeka J, Carver S, Allison L, Kiemel T (2005) Dynamics of sensory reweighting: time scales, transient and asymptotic dynamics. IV Conference on Motor control MCC2005—from basic motor control to functional recovery, Sofia

  • Jeka J, Allison L, Saffer M, Zhang Y, Carver S, Kiemel T (2006) Sensory reweighting with translational visual stimuli in young and elderly adults: the role of state-dependent noise. Exp Brain Res 174:517–527

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kiemel T, Oie KS, Jeka JJ (2002) Multisensory fusion and the stochastic structure of postural sway. Biol Cybern 87:262–277

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim S (2004) The use of vision in children’s postural control. Master Thesis, University of Maryland, College Park

  • Kuo AD (1995) An optimal-control model for analyzing human postural balance. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 42:87–101

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo AD (2005) An optimal state estimation model of sensory integration in human postural balance. J Neural Eng 2:S235–S249

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner JR (1992) Multimodal and motor influences on orientation: implications for adapting to weightless and virtual environments. J Vestib Res 2:307–322

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN, Aronson E (1974) Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants. Percept Psychophys 15:529–532

    Google Scholar 

  • Mergner T, Maurer C, Peterka RJ (2003) A multisensory posture control model of human upright stance. Prog Brain Res 142:189–201

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe JS, Clark JE (2000) Somatosensory information affords the exploration of posture in newly walking infants and toddlers. Infant Behav Dev 23:391–405

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe JS, Chen LC, Chang TY, McDowell K, Jeka JJ, Clark JE (2005a) The temporal organization of posture changes during the first year of independent walking. Exp Brain Res 161:405–416

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe JS, McDowell K, Chang TY, Chen LC, Jeka JJ, Clark JE (2005b) Development of somatosensory-motor integration: an event-related analysis of infant posture in the first year of independent walking. Dev Psychobiol 46:19–35

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oie KS, Kiemel T, Jeka JJ (2002) Multisensory fusion: simultaneous re-weighting of vision and touch for the control of human posture. Cogn Brain Res 14:164–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterka RJ, Benolken MS (1995) Role of somatosensory and vestibular cues in attenuating visually induced human postural sway. Exp Brain Res 105:101–110

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peterka RJ (2002) Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol 88:1097–1118

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Polit DF (1996) Data analysis & statistics for nursing research. Appleton & Lange, Stamford

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravaioli E, Oie KS, Kiemel T, Chiari L, Jeka JJ (2005) Nonlinear postural control in response to visual translation. Exp Brain Res 160:450–459

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rock I, Harris CS (1967) Vision and touch. Sci Am 216:96–104

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schmuckler MA (1997) Children’s postural sway in response to low- and high-frequency visual information for oscillation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23:528–545

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Seber GAF (1984) Multivariate observations. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Seber GAF, Wild CJ (2003) Nonlinear regression. Wiley-Interscience, Hoboken

    Google Scholar 

  • Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott MH (1985) The growth of stability: postural control from a developmental perspective. J Motor Behav 17:131–147

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van der Kooij H, Jacobs R, Koopman B, Grootenboer H (1999) A multisensory integration model of human stance control. Biol Cybern 80:299–308

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van der Kooij H, Jacobs R, Koopman B, van der Helm F (2001) An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic environment applied to human stance control. Biol Cybern 84:103–115

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Woollacott MH, Debû B, Mowatt M (1987) Neuromuscular control of posture in the infant and child: is vision dominant? J Motor Behav 19:167–186

    CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This project is supported by NIH HD42527 to Dr. Jane E. Clark and a scholarship from the Taiwan Ministry of Education to Woei-Nan Bair.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jane E. Clark.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bair, WN., Kiemel, T., Jeka, J.J. et al. Development of multisensory reweighting for posture control in children. Exp Brain Res 183, 435–446 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1057-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1057-2

Keywords

Navigation