Skip to main content
Log in

Alleviating the ‘crossed-hands’ deficit by seeing uncrossed rubber hands

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Localizing and reacting to tactile events on our skin requires the coordination between primary somatotopic projections and an external representation of space. Previous research has attributed an important role to early visual experience in shaping up this mapping. Here, we addressed the role played by immediately available visual information about body posture. We asked participants to determine the temporal order of two successive tactile events delivered to the hands while they adopted a crossed or an uncrossed-hands posture. As previously found, hand-crossing led to a dramatic impairment in tactile localization, which is a phenomenon attributed to a mismatch between somatotopic and externally-based frames of reference. In the present study, however, participants watched a pair of rubber hands that were placed either in a crossed or uncrossed posture (congruent or incongruent with the posture of their own hands). The results showed that the crossed-hands deficit can be significantly ameliorated by the sight of uncrossed rubber hands (Experiment 1). Moreover, this visual modulation seemed to depend critically on the degree to which the visual information about the rubber hands can be attributed to one’s own actions, in a process revealing short-term adaptation (Experiment 2).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Equation 1
Fig. 2
Equation 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The body is in a typical posture when the left limbs are placed on the left of the body and vice versa for the right limbs.

  2. In fact, in order to confirm this, we also explored the data using a 2-parameter model. The analysis of this condition results in a very significant, but clearly artifactual, difference in terms of time constant (P < 0.001).

  3. Statement #1 (“I felt as if my hands were uncrossed”); #2 (“It seemed as if the hand which pressed the top button was my real hand”) #3 (“I felt as if the rubber hands were my hands”) #4 (“It appeared as if my hands (under the box) were displacing towards the top (above the box)”), #5 (“It seemed as if I might have two right hands or two left hands”), #6 (“I felt as if my (real) hands were turning ‘rubbery’”) and #7 (“It appeared (visually) as if the rubber hands (on the top of the box) were displacing towards the bottom (towards my real hands)”).

Abbreviations

JND:

Just noticeable difference

LED:

Light-emitting diode

PSS:

Point of subjective simultaneity

SI:

Primary somatosensory cortex

SOA:

Stimulus onset asynchrony

TOJ:

Temporal order judgement

References

  • Aglioti S, Smania N, Peru A (1999) Frames of reference for mapping tactile stimuli in brain-damaged patients. J Cogn Neurosci 11(1):67–79

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Armel KC, Ramachandran VS (2003) Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response. Proc Biol Sci 270(1523):1499–1506

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Austen EL, Soto-Faraco S, Enns JT, Kingstone A (2004) Mislocalizations of touch to a fake hand. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4(2):170–181

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Batista AP, Buneo CA, Snyder LH, Andersen RA (1999) Reach plans in eye-centered coordinates. Science 285(5425):257–260

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Benedetti F (1985) Processing of tactile spatial information with crossed fingers. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 11:517–525

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Benedetti F (1988) Exploration of a rod with crossed fingers. Percept Psychophys 44:281–284

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berti A, Frassinetti F (2000) When far becomes near: remapping of space by tool use. J Cogn Neurosci 12:415–420

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick M, Cohen J (1998) Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature 391(6669):756

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Craig JC (2003) The effect of hand position and pattern motion in temporal order judgements. Percept Psychophys 65(5):779–788

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craig JC, Belser AN (2006) The crossed-hands deficit in tactile temporal-order judgments: the effect of training. Perception 35:1561–1572

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Driver J, Grossenbacher PG (1996) Multimodal spatial constraints on tactile selective attention. In: Inui T, McClelland JL (eds) Attention and performance XVI: information integration in perception and communication. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 209–235

    Google Scholar 

  • Efron B, Tibshirani RJ (1993) An introduction to the bootstrap. Chapman and Hall, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrsson HH, Holmes NP, Passingham RE (2005) Touching a rubber hand: feeling of body ownership is associated with activity in multisensory brain areas. J Neurosci 25(45):10564–10573

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Facchini S, Aglioti SM (2003) Short term light deprivation increases tactile spatial acuity in humans. Neurology 60(12):1998–1999

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Farnè A, Làdavas E (2000) Dynamic size-change of hand peripersonal space following tool use. Neuroreport 11:1645–1649

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallace A, Spence C (2005) Visual capture of apparent limb position influences tactile temporal order judgments. Neurosci Lett 379(1):63–68

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Henri V (1898) Über die Raumwahrnehmung des Tastsinnes: Ein Beitrag zur experimentellen Psychologie [On the spatial perception of the tactile sense: A contribution to experimental psychology]. Reuther & Reichard, Berlin

  • Holmes NP, Spence C (2004) The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cogn Process 5(2):94–105

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes NP, Spence C (2005) Visual bias of unseen hand position with a mirror: spatial and temporal factors. Exp Brain Res 166(3–4):489–497

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes NP, Calvert GA, Spence C (2004) Extending or projecting peripersonal space with tools? Multisensory interactions highlight only the distal and proximal ends of tools. Neurosci Lett 372:62–67

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Iriki A, Tanaka M, Iwamura Y (1996) Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. Neuroreport 7(14):2325–2330

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kanai R, Sheth BR, Shimojo S (2004) Stopping the motion and perceptual mislocalization. Vision Res 44:2605–2619

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kauffman T, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A (2002) Braille character discrimination in blindfolded human subjects. Neuroreport 13(5):571–574

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kitazawa S (2002) Where conscious sensation takes place. Conscious Cogn 11:475–477

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kóbor I, Furedi L, Kovacs G, Spence C, Vidnyanszky Z (2006) Back-to-front: improved tactile discrimination performance in the space you cannot see. Neurosci Lett 400(1–2):163–167

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos S, Shepard RN (1997) Time–distance relations in shifting attention between locations on one’s body. Percept Psychophys 59:557–566

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd D (2007) Spatial limits on referred touch to an alien limb may reflect boundaries of visuo-tactile peripersonal space surrounding the hand. Brain Cogn (in press)

  • Lloyd D, Morrison I, Roberts N (2006) Role for human posterior parietal cortex in visual processing of aversive objects in peripersonal space. J Neurophysiol 95(1):205–214

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita A, Iriki A (2004) Tools for the body (schema). Trends Cogn Sci 8:79–86

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita A, Husain M, Clarke K, Driver J (2001) Reaching with a tool extends visual–tactile interactions into far space: evidence from cross-modal extinction. Neuropsychologia 39:580–585

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita A, Clarke K, Husain M, Driver J (2002a) Active tool use with the contralesional hand can reduce cross-modal extinction of touch on that hand. Neurocase 8:411–416

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita A, Spence C, Kennett S, Driver J (2002b) Tool-use changes multimodal spatial interactions between vision and touch in normal humans. Cognition 83:B25–B34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita A, Spence C, Sergent C, Driver J (2002c) Seeing your own touched hands in a mirror modulates cross-modal interactions. Psychol Sci 13(4):350–355

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moscovitch M, Behrmann M (1994) Coding of spatial information in the somatosensory system: evidence from patients with neglect following parietal lobe damage. J Cogn Neurosci 6:151–155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pallier C, Dupoux E, Jeannin X (1997) Expe5: an expandable programming language for on-line psychological experiments. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 29:322–327

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavani F, Spence C, Driver J (2000) Visual capture of touch: out-of-the-body experiences with rubber gloves. Psychol Sci 11(5):353–359

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Penfield W, Rasmussen T (1950) The cerebral cortex of man: a clinical study of localization of function. Hafner, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ponzo M (1910) Intorno ad alcune illusioni nel campo delle sensazioni tattili, sull’illusione di Aristotele e fenomeni analoghi [On some tactile illusions, Aristotle’s illusion, and similar phenomena]. Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie 16:307–345

    Google Scholar 

  • Pouget A, Ducom JC, Torri J, Bavelier D (2002) Multisensory spatial representations in eye-centered coordinates for reaching. Cognition 83(1):B1–B11

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rinker MA, Craig JC (1994) The effect of spatial orientation on the perception of moving tactile stimuli. Percept Psychophys 56:356–362

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Röder B, Rösler F, Spence C (2004) Early vision impairs tactile perception in the blind. Curr Biol 14:121–124

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schicke T, Röder B (2006) Spatial remapping of touch: confusion of perceived stimulus order across hand and foot. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103(31):11808–11813

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shore DI, Spry E, Spence C (2002) Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. Cogn Brain Res 14(1):153–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shore DI, Gray K, Spry E, Spence C (2005) Spatial modulation of tactile temporal-order judgments. Perception 34:1251–1262

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smania N, Aglioti S (1995) Sensory and spatial components of somaesthesic deficits following right brain damage. Neurology 45:1725–1730

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Soto-Faraco S, Ronald A, Spence C (2004) Tactile selective attention and body posture: assessing the multisensory contributions of vision and proprioception. Percept Psychophys 66(7):1077–1094

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence C, Baddeley R, Zampini M, James R, Shore DI (2003) Multisensory temporal order judgments: when two locations are better than one. Percept Psychophys 65(2):318–328

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland MT (2006) The hand and the ipsilateral primary Somatosensory Cortex. J Neurosci 26(32):8217–8218

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper SP, Phillips N, Dancer C, Lloyd D, Howard LA, McGlone F (2001) Vision influences tactile perception at body sites that cannot be viewed directly. Exp Brain Res 39:160–167

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wada M, Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2004) Effects of handedness on tactile temporal order judgment. Neuropsychologia 42:1887–1895

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S (2001) Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing. Nat Neurosci 4:759–765

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zampini M, Brown T, Shore DI, Maravita A, Röder B, Spence C (2005a) Audiotactile temporal order judgments. Acta Psychol (Amst) 118(3):277–291

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zampini M, Harris C, Spence C (2005b) Effect of posture change on tactile perception: impaired direction discrimination performance with interleaved fingers. Exp Brain Res 166:498–508

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia TIN2004-04363-C03-02. E.A. is supported by a fellowship Beca de Formación de Profesorado Universitario from the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia. We would like to thank Joan López-Moliner for his valuable help with the data analyses. We would also like to thank Mikel Santesteban and Aida Mallorquí.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Salvador Soto-Faraco.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Azañón, E., Soto-Faraco, S. Alleviating the ‘crossed-hands’ deficit by seeing uncrossed rubber hands. Exp Brain Res 182, 537–548 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1011-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-007-1011-3

Keywords

Navigation