Skip to main content
Log in

Time-on-task effect in pseudoneglect

  • Research Note
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Neurologically normal subjects systematically misbisect space during visual line-bisection or similar tasks, generally erring to the left of the veridical center when bisecting horizontal lines, a phenomenon referred to as pseudoneglect. This phenomenon is usually interpreted as enhanced attention toward the left hemispace resulting in an overestimation of the leftward extent of a line. While most studies have examined the role of attention in spatial bias using spatial cueing methods in bisection tasks, Manly et al. (Neuropsychologia 43(12):1721–1728, 2005) proposed an original paradigm in which the participants’ alertness was diminished by sleep deprivation or prolonged execution of a line-bisection task. The authors reported a significant rightward shift in attention related to declining alertness, but they did not control eye movements and, consequently, modifications of scanning and fixation strategies with fatigue cannot be ruled out in their study. Here we examine whether a diminution in alertness induced by a 60-min-long Landmark task would diminish (or even reverse) this attentional bias, when eye movements are absent. Participants performed a forced-choice judgment about the location of a transaction mark in relation to the veridical center of a horizontal line. The results confirmed a significant decrease in the leftward bias over the course of the session but, in contrast to the findings of Manly et al. (2005), we did not observe a reverse bias from the left to the right hemispace. The results are discussed within the context of the hemisphere-activation model.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akerstedt T, Gillberg M (1990) Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 52(1–2):29–37

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Arguin M, Bub DN (1993) Evidence for an independent stimulus-centered spatial reference frame from a case of visual hemineglect. Cortex 29(2):349–357

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Billingsley RL, Simos PG, Sarkari S, Fletcher JM, Papanicolaou AC (2004) Spatio-temporal brain activation profiles associated with line bisection judgments and double simultaneous visual stimulation. Behav Brain Res 152(1):97–107

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Binder J, Marshall R, Lazar R, Benjamin J, Mohr JP (1992) Distinct syndromes of hemineglect. Arch Neurol 49(11):1187–1194

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach E, Capitani E, Porta E (1985) Two basic properties of space representation in the brain: evidence from unilateral neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 48(2):141–144

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Boller F, Howes D, Patten DH (1970) A behavioral evaluation of brain-scan estimates of lesion size. Neurology 20(9):852–859

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers D, Heilman KM (1980) Pseudoneglect: effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia 18(4–5):491–498

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw JL, Bradshaw JA, Nathan G, Nettleton NC, Wilson LE (1986) Leftwards error in bisecting the gap between two points: stimulus quality and hand effects. Neuropsychologia 24(6):849–855

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bradshaw JL, Nathan G, Nettleton NC, Wilson L, Pierson J (1987) Why is there a left side underestimation in rod bisection? Neuropsychologia 25(4):735–738

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brodie EE, Pettigrew LE (1996) Is left always right? Directional deviations in visual line bisection as a function of hand and initial scanning direction. Neuropsychologia 34(5):467–470

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cabungcal JH, Misslisch H, Scherberger H, Hepp K, Hess BJ (2001) Effect of light sleep on three-dimensional eye position in static roll and pitch. Vis Res 41(4):495–505

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chokron S, Bartolomeo P, Perenin MT, Helft G, Imbert M (1998) Scanning direction and line bisection: a study of normal subjects and unilateral neglect patients with opposite reading habits. Cogn Brain Res 7(2):173–178

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, Drury HA et al (1998) A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21(4):761–773

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Dellatolas G, Vanluchene J, Coutin T (1996) Visual and motor components in simple line bisection: an investigation in normal adults. Cogn Brain Res 4(1):49–56

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fink GR, Marshall JC, Shah NJ, Weiss PH, Halligan PW, Grosse-Ruyken M et al (2000) Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI. Neurology 54(6):1324–1331

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fink GR, Marshall JC, Weiss PH, Zilles K (2001) The neural basis of vertical and horizontal line bisection judgments: an fMRI study of normal volunteers. Neuroimage 14(1Pt2):S59–S67

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fink GR, Marshall JC, Weiss PH, Toni I, Zilles K (2002) Task instructions influence the cognitive strategies involved in line bisection judgements: evidence from modulated neural mechanisms revealed by fMRI. Neuropsychologia 40(2):119–130

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Finney DJ (1962) Probit analysis. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Foxe JJ, McCourt ME, Javitt DC (2003) Right hemisphere control of visuospatial attention: line-bisection judgments evaluated with high-density electrical mapping and source analysis. Neuroimage 19(3):710–726

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey M, Milner AD, Roberts RC (1995) An investigation of hemispatial neglect using the Landmark Task. Brain Cogn 27(1):59–78

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T (1980) Right hemisphere dominance for attention: the mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology 30(3):327–330

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ishiai S, Furukawa T, Tsukagoshi H (1989) Visuospatial processes of line bisection and the mechanisms underlying unilateral spatial neglect. Brain 112(Pt 6):1485–1502

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kerkhoff G (2001) Spatial hemineglect in humans. Prog Neurobiol 63(1):1–27

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne M (1970) The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychol 33:193–201

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lavine RA, Sibert JL, Gokturk M, Dickens B (2002) Eye-tracking measures and human performance in a vigilance task. Aviat Space Environ Med 73(4):367–372

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin JS, Friedman L, Wu D, Miller DA, Thompson LA, Klein SK et al (1996) Cortical localization of human sustained attention: detection with functional MR using a visual vigilance paradigm. J Comput Assist Tomogr 20(5):695–701

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Manly T, Dobler VB, Dodds CM, George MA (2005) Rightward shift in spatial awareness with declining alertness. Neuropsychologia 43(12):1721–1728

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall RS, Lazar RM, Van Heertum RL, Esser PD, Perera GM, Mohr JP (1997) Changes in regional cerebral blood flow related to line bisection discrimination and visual attention using HMPAO-SPECT. Neuroimage 6(2):139–144

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt ME (2001) Performance consistency of normal observers in forced-choice tachistoscopic visual line bisection. Neuropsychologia 39(10):1065–1076

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt ME, Freeman P, Tahmahkera-Stevens C, Chaussee M (2001) The influence of unimanual response on pseudoneglect magnitude. Brain Cogn 45(1):52–63

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McCourt ME, Garlinghouse M, Reuter-Lorenz PA (2005) Unilateral visual cueing and asymmetric line geometry share a common attentional origin in the modulation of pseudoneglect. Cortex 41(4):499–511

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mennemeier M, Vezey E, Chatterjee A, Rapcsak SZ, Heilman KM (1997) Contributions of the left and right cerebral hemispheres to line bisection. Neuropsychologia 35(5):703–715

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mesulam MM (1981) A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 10(4):309–325

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Milner AD, Brechmann M, Pagliarini L (1992) To halve and to halve not: an analysis of line bisection judgements in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 30(6):515–526

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moller C, Odkvist L, White V, Cyr D (1990) The plasticity of compensatory eye movements in rotatory tests. I. The effect of alertness and eye closure. Acta Oto-Laryngol 109(1–2):15–24

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nobre AC, Gitelman DR, Dias EC, Mesulam MM (2000) Covert visual spatial orienting and saccades: overlapping neural systems. Neuroimage 11(3):210–216

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9(1):97–113

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pardo JV, Fox PT, Raichle ME (1991) Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature 349(6304):61–64

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Driver J (1992) The neurobiology of selective attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2(2):165–169

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reuter-Lorenz PA, Kinsbourne M, Moscovitch M (1990) Hemispheric control of spatial attention. Brain Cogn 12(2):240–266

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson IH, North NT, Geggie C (1992) Spatiomotor cueing in unilateral left neglect: three case studies of its therapeutic effects. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55(9):799–805

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schiff BB, Truchon C (1993) Effect of unilateral contraction of hand muscles on perceiver biases in the perception of chimeric and neutral faces. Neuropsychologia 31(12):1351–1365

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki Y, Straumann D, Henn V (2000) Effects of alertness on three-dimensional eye movements. Jpn J Ophthalmol 44(5):457–462

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vingiano W (1991) Pseudoneglect on a cancellation task. Int J Neurosci 58(1–2):63–67

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to André Dufour.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dufour, A., Touzalin, P. & Candas, V. Time-on-task effect in pseudoneglect. Exp Brain Res 176, 532–537 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0810-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0810-2

Keywords

Navigation