Skip to main content
Log in

The acquisition and implementation of the smoothness maximization motion strategy is dependent on spatial accuracy demands

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We recently showed that extensive training on a sequence of planar hand trajectories passing through several targets resulted in the co-articulation of movement components and in the formation of new movement elements (primitives) (Sosnik et al. in Exp Brain Res 156(4):422–438, 2004). Reduction in movement duration was accompanied by the gradual replacing of a piecewise combination of rectilinear trajectories with a single, longer curved one, the latter affording the maximization of movement smoothness (“global motion planning”). The results from transfer experiments, conducted by the end of the last training session, have suggested that the participants have acquired movement elements whose attributes were solely dictated by the figural (i.e., geometrical) form of the path, rather than by both path geometry and its time derivatives. Here we show that the acquired movement generation strategy (“global motion planning”) was not specific to the trained configuration or total movement duration. Performance gain (i.e., movement smoothness, defined by the fit of the data to the behavior, predicted by the “global planning” model) transferred to non-trained configurations in which the targets were spatially co-aligned or when participants were instructed to perform the task in a definite amount of time. Surprisingly, stringent accuracy demands, in transfer conditions, resulted not only in an increased movement duration but also in reverting to the straight trajectories (loss of co-articulation), implying that the performance gain was dependent on accuracy constraints. Only 28.5% of the participants (two out of seven) who were trained in the absence of visual feedback from the hand (dark condition) co-articulated by the end of the last training session compared to 75% (six out of eight) who were trained in the light, and none of them has acquired a geometrical motion primitive. Furthermore, six naïve participants who trained in dark condition on large size targets have all co-articulated by the end of the last training session, still, none of them has acquired a geometrical motion primitive. Taken together, our results indicate that the acquisition of a geometrical motion primitive is dependent on the existence of visual feedback from the hand and that the implementation of the smoothness-maximization motion strategy is dependent on spatial accuracy demands. These findings imply that the specific features of the training experience (i.e., temporal or spatial task demands) determine the attributes of an acquired motion planning strategy and primitive.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abend W, Bizzi E, Morasso P (1982) Human arm trajectory formation. Brain 105:331–348

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander RM (1997) Minimum energy cost hypothesis for human arm trajectories. Biol Cybern 76:97–105

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Berthier NE (1996) Learning to reach: a mathematical model. Dev Psych 32:811–823

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bizzi E, Tresch MC, Saltiel P, d’Avella (2000) A New perspectives on spinal motor systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:101–108

  • Blackburn CS, Young S (2000) A self-learning predictive model of articulator movements during speech production. J Acoust Soc Am 107(3):1659–1670

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cruse E (1986) Constraints for joint angle control of the human arm. Biol Cybern 54:125–132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel KC, Flanders M, Soechting JF (1997) Anticipatory and sequential motor control in piano playing. Exp Brain Res 113:189–199

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan JR, Rao AK (1995) Trajectory adaptation to nonlinear visuomotor transformation: evidence of motion planning in visually perceived space. J Neurosci 74(5):2174–2177

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flash T, Gurevich I (1992) Human motor adaptation to external loads. Ann Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 13:885–886

    Google Scholar 

  • Flash T, Henis EA (1991) Arm trajectory modification during reaching towards visual targets. J Cog Neurosci 3:220–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flash T, Hogan N (1985) The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J Neurosci 5(7):1688–1703

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flash T, Henis E, Inzelberg R, Korczyn AD (1992) Timing and sequencing of human arm trajectories: normal and abnormal motor behavior. Hum Mov Sci 11:83–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghahramani Z, Wolpert DM (1997) Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning. Nature 386:392–395

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ghilardi MF, Gordon J, Ghez C (1995) Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of workspace produces directional biases in other areas. J Neurophysiol 73:2535–2539

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1994) Accuracy of planar reaching movements – I. Independence of direction and extended variability. Exp Brain Res 99:97–111

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gribble PL, Ostry DJ, Sanguineti V, Laboissiere R (1998) Are complex control signals required for human arm movement? J Neurophysiol 79:1409–1424

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hardcastle W, Marchal A (1990) Speech production and speech modeling. Kluwer, Dordrecht

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris CM, Wolpert DM (1998) Signal dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394:780–784

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hasan Z (1986) Optimized movement trajectories and joint stiffness in unperturbated, inertially loaded movement. Biol Cybern 53:373–382

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hofsten C (1991) Structuring of early reaching movements: a longitudinal study. J Mot Behav 23:280–292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollerbach JM, Flash T (1982) Dynamic interaction between limb segments during planar arm movements. Biol Cybern 44:67–77

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs S, Hanneton S, Heude S, Roby-Brami A (2003) Is the velocity-curvature relationship disrupted in apraxic patients? Neuro Rep 14(15):1907–1911

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jerde TE, Soechting JF, Flanders M (2003) Coarticulation in fluent finger spelling. J Neurosci 23(6):2383–2393

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Karniel A, Inbar GF (1997) A model for learning human reaching movements. Biol Cybern 77:173–183

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Karni A, Meyer G, Rey-Hipolito C, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, Ungerleider LG (1998) The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 95:861–868

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kent RD, Minifie FD (1977) Coarticulation in recent speech production models. J Phon 5:115–117

    Google Scholar 

  • Konczak J, Borutta M, Topka H, Dichgans J (1995) The development of goal-directed reaching in infants: hand trajectory formation and joint force control. Exp Brain Res 106:156–168

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Korman M, Raz N, Flash T, Karni A (2003) Multiple shifts in the representation of a motor sequence during the acquisition of skilled performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100(21):12492–12497

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs HI, Aisen ML, Volpe BT, Hogan N (1999) Quantization of continuous arm movements in humans with brain injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:4645–4649

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krylow AM, Rymer WZ (1997) Role of intrinsic muscle properties in producing smooth movements. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 44:165–176

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lan N (1997) Analysis of an optimal control model of multi-joint arm movements. Biol Cybern 76:107–117

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • MacNeilage PF (1980) Speech production. Lang Speech 23:3–22

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McIntyre J, Stratta F, Lacquaniti F (1997) Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. J Neurophysiol 78:1601–1618

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Messier J, Kalaska JF (1997) Differential effects of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 115:469–478

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Messier J, Kalaska JF (1999) Comparison of variability of initial kinematics and endpoints of reaching movements. Exp Brain Res 125:139–152

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Morasso P (1981) Spatial control of arm movements. Exp Brain Res 43:223–227

    Google Scholar 

  • Morasso P, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (1982) Trajectory formation and handwriting: a computational model. Biol Cybern 45(2):131–42

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Bizzi E (2000) Motor learning through the combination of primitives. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 355:1755–1769

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Polyakov F, Flash T, Abeles M, Ben Shaul Y, Drori R, Nadasdy Z (2001) Analysis of motion planning and learning in monkey scribbling movements. In: Proceedings of the tenth biennial conference of the international graphonomics society 78:83

  • Rohrer B, Fasoli S, Krebs HI, Hughes R, Volpe B, Frontera WR, Stein J, Hogan N (2002) Movement smoothness changes during stroke recovery. J Neurosci 22(18):8297–304

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saltiel P, Wyler-Duda K, d’Avella A, Tresch M, Bizzi E (2001) Muscle synergies encoded within the spinal cord: evidence from focal intraspinal NMDA iontophoresis in the frog. J Neurophysiol. 85:605–619

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Santello M, Flanders M, Soechting JF (1998) Postural hand synergies for tool use. J Neurosci 18:10105–10115

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shadmehr R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (1994) Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. J Neurosci 14:3208–3224

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Bueno CA, Hermann U, Flanders M (1995) Moving effortlessly in three dimensions: does Denders law applies to arm movements? J Neurosci 15:6271–6280

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sosnik R, Hauptmann B, Karni A, Flash T (2004) When practice leads to co-articulation: the evolution of geometrically defined movement primitives. Exp Brain Res 156(4):422–438

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov E, Jordan M (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nature Neurosci 5(11):1226:1235

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov E, Jordan M (2003) In: Becker S, Thrun S, Obermayer K (eds) Advances in neural information processing system, vol 15. MIT, Cambridge, pp 27–34

  • Torres E, Zipser D (2002) Reaching to grasp with a multi-jointed arm. I. Computational model. J Neurophysiol 88:2355–2367

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uno Y, Kawato M, Ito K (1989) Formation and control of human trajectory in human multi joint arm movement—minimum torque change model. Biol Cybern 61:89–101

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Van den Dobbelsteen JJ, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2001) Endpoints of arm movements to visual targets. Exp Brain Res 138:279:287

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Beers RJ, Haggard P, Wolpert DM (2004) The role of execution noise in movement variability. J Neurophysiol 91:1050–1063

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vindras P, Viviani P (1998) Frames of references and control parameters in visuomanual pointing. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:569–591

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Viviani P, Flash T (1995) Minimum-jerk, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 21(1):32–53

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss EJ, Flanders M (2004) Muscular and postural synergies of the human hand. J. Neurophysiol 92(1):523–535

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z, Jordan MI (1995) Are arm trajectories planned in kinematic or dynamic coordinates? An adaptation study. Exp Brain Res 103:460–470

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • d’Avella A, Santiel P, Bizzi E (2003) Contributions of muscle synergies in the construction of natural motor behavior. Nat Neurosci 6:300–308

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by the Einhorn-Dominic Foundation, by the DIP grant to Prof Tamar Flash and by the Morros laboratory. Prof. Tamar Flash is an incumbent of Dr. Hymie Morros professorial chair.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ronen Sosnik.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sosnik, R., Flash, T., Hauptmann, B. et al. The acquisition and implementation of the smoothness maximization motion strategy is dependent on spatial accuracy demands. Exp Brain Res 176, 311–331 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0617-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0617-1

Keywords

Navigation