Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Pronounced overestimation of support surface tilt during stance

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A veridical internal notion of the kinematic state of the foot support is essential for postural control. The means by which this is obtained is still a matter of debate. We therefore measured the conscious perception of support tilt during transient anterior–posterior rotations of a motion platform in six healthy subjects, using a psychophysical matching procedure. Furthermore, we evaluated subjects’ postural responses (in terms of displacement of subjects’ center of mass, COM, and their ankle torque, as represented by the center of foot pressure, COP). The platform tilts were applied in absence of visual and auditory orientation cues. The platform rotations consisted of smoothed position ramps with different dominant frequencies (0.025, 0.05, 0.1, 0.2, 0.4, and 0.8 Hz) and different amplitudes (0.125°, 0.25°, 0.5°, 1°, 2°, 4°, and 8°) for the forward and backward directions, which yielded a 6×14 stimulus matrix. The stimuli were repeated five times in a random order. For the matching procedure, subjects tried to maintain an upright body orientation, while trying to orient a light-weight rod, which was attached to a belt around their waists, parallel to the perceived platform surface. We measured the stimulus-evoked angular excursions of the rod and of the subjects’ COM as well as the COP shift. We found that the subjects’ rod indications overestimated the platform tilts, particularly with small stimulus amplitudes. To characterize the overestimation, we compared the rod indications obtained while subjects stood on the tilting platform, to rod indications in a situation in which they stood next to the platform and tried to match the rod angle to the now visually perceived platform angle. From this comparison, we inferred that the subjects’ kinesthetically derived notion of platform tilt overestimates the actual tilt by a factor of ≈4. The estimates were linearly related to the angle between body (COM) and platform, i.e., to approximately the angle of the ankle joint, a finding which suggests a proprioceptive source of the overestimation. Further analyses supported this view; they showed that the onset latencies of the rod indications could be approximated by a theoretical indication mechanism with a reaction time of about 0.31 s, a velocity threshold of 0.099°/s, and a displacement threshold of 0.12°. These threshold values are well in line with previous work on the leg proprioceptive detection threshold of conscious perception of body sway. We therefore assume that the phenomenon of support tilt overestimation reflects a still unknown mechanism of leg proprioception in postural control.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. With this stimulus, the reaction time for a position threshold of 0.125° would be 0.17 s, and a combination of this position threshold with the 0.1°/s velocity threshold would yield 0.19 s. This value is clearly below the experimentally observed time delay of 0.52 s.

  2. The mean values in Fig. 6a, b, f, g depended somewhat on stimulus frequency, which we omitted here, however, in view of the following comparisons with subjects’ indications which were essentially independent of frequency.

  3. Let us consider a simple postural control model of the body in space (BS) in terms of an inverted pendulum, which is accelerated by gravity (g). The torque evoked in the ankle joints depends in addition on body mass (m), the height (h) of the mass above the joints, and BS angle (i.e., mgh* sin(BS)). The angle BS is sensed and an estimate of it is used as a negative feedback signal to keep the body (pendulum) upright. The response is achieved with the help of a neural controller which consists of a stiffness (proportional control, K P) and a damping (differential control, K D;PD control). We now feed into this system a command signal for a voluntary body lean of 1° forward, let us say. If fed into the model in direct form at a site where the sensory information of BS forms the error signal, the achieved body offset from primary position would yield 4° instead of 1°. The reason is that the value of K P is usually about 1/3 larger than mgh, that is, K P equals about 4/3 * mgh (see Peterka 2002). The DC gain (steady state) of the system is then equal to K P/(K Pmgh) = 4. The error can be omitted in the model, for instance by amplifying the command signal by a factor of 4

References

  • Berenberg RA, Shefner JM, Sabol JJ Jr (1987) Quantitative assessment of position sense at the ankle: a functional approach. Neurology 37:89–93

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bressel E, Larsen BT, McNair PJ, Cronin J (2004) Ankle joint proprioception and passive mechanical properties of the calf muscles after an Achilles tendon rupture: a comparison with matched controls. Clin Biomech 19:284–291

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark FJ, Burgess RC, Chapin JW, Lipscomb WT (1985) Role of intramuscular receptors in the awareness of limb position. J Neurophysiol 54:1529–1540

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • van Deursen RW, Sanchez MM, Ulbrecht JS, Cavanagh PR (1998) The role of muscle spindles in ankle movement perception in human subjects with diabetic neuropathy. Exp Brain Res 120:1–8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzpatrick R, McCloskey DI (1994) Proprioceptive, visual and vestibular thresholds for the perception of sway during standing in humans. J Physiol 478:173–186

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzpatrick RC, Taylor JL, McCloskey DI (1992) Ankle stiffness of standing humans in response to imperceptible perturbation: reflex and task-dependent components. J Physiol 454:533–547

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hlavacka F, Mergner T, Bolha B (1996) Human self-motion perception during translatory vestibular and proprioceptive stimulation. Neurosci Lett 210:83–86

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lagarias JC, Reeds JA, Wright MH, Wright PE (1998) Convergence properties of the Nelder-Mead simplex method in low dimensions. SIAM J Optim 9:112–147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer C, Mergner T, Bolha B, Hlavacka F (2000) Vestibular, visual, and somatosensory contributions to human control of upright stance. Neursci Lett 281:99–102

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer C, Mergner T, Bolha B, Hlavacka F (2001) Human balance control during cutaneous stimulation of the plantar soles. Neurosci Lett 302:45–48

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mergner T, Siebold C, Schweigart G, Becker W (1991) Human perception of horizontal head and trunk rotation in space during vestibular and neck stimulation. Exp Brain Res 85:389–404

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mergner T, Hlavacka F, Schweigart G (1993) Interaction of vestibular and proprioceptive inputs. J Vestib Res 3:41–57

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Peterka RJ (2002) Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol 88:1097–1118

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Refshauge KM, Fitzpatrick RC (1995) Perception of movement at the human ankle: effects of leg position. J Physiol 488:243–248

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schweigart G, Mergner T, Barnes GR (2003) Object motion perception is shaped by the motor control mechanism of ocular pursuit. Exp Brain Res 148:350–365

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Soechting JF, Ross B (1984) Psychophysical determination of coordinate representation of human arm orientation. Neuroscience 13:595–604

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale N, Nougier V, Barraud PA, Bourdin C, Debu B, Poquin D, Raphel C (1999) Contribution of ankle, knee, and hip joints to the perception threshold for support surface rotation. Percept Psychophys 61:615–624

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, DA (1990) Biomechanics and motor control of human movement, 2nd edn, Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Me715/5–2.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to C. Maurer.

Appendix

Appendix

When analyzing the time delay values, which occurred between the stimulus onset and the beginning of the rod indication, we found that neither the detection time of a velocity threshold alone nor a combination of it with a position threshold could explain the experimental findings. Furthermore, there appeared to be a minimal time delay with the highest frequency of the platform tilts tested, indicating that a rather fixed reaction time is contained, in addition to the total time delay between stimulus onset and response. These considerations led us to determine in an parameter identification procedure, estimates of position and velocity threshold values as well as reaction time values that could optimally account for the time-delay pattern. The parameter identification procedure varied the values for the thresholds and the reaction time. We used the Matlab Optimization toolbox function “fminsearch” to find the minimum of a scalar error function of the time delay pattern, starting at an initial estimate. The algorithm ‘fminsearch’ uses the simplex search method of Nelder–Mead (Lagarias et al. 1998). We started with zero threshold values and zero reaction time as initial estimates. With each iteration of the parameter identification procedure, the current threshold and reaction time values were applied to all 84 platform trajectories to determine a new pattern of time delays, and a scalar error function was evaluated. Then, the parameter identification procedure made changes to the threshold and reaction time values, a new pattern of time delays was calculated, and the error function re-evaluated. This sequence was repeated until the error function was minimized. In order to normalize the error contribution from each data point of the time-delay pattern and to achieve a symmetrical error distribution around the reference values, we used the error function:

$$E = \sum\limits_{i = 1}^N {\left({\left| {\frac{{T_i - \hat T_i}}{{T_i + \hat T_i}}} \right|} \right)} $$

where N is the number of data points of the pattern of time delays (N = 84), T i is the experimentally measured reference pattern of time delays, and \(\hat T_i \) is the estimated pattern of time delays, obtained with each iteration of the parameter identification procedure. This procedure is based on the assumption that the velocity and position thresholds were parallel and independent. When assuming a serial assembly of a velocity first and then a position threshold, the identified velocity threshold and reaction time were similar, but the position threshold was 3% lower compared to the parallel assembly. A serial assembly of first position threshold and then velocity threshold led to a reduction of the velocity threshold by 4%.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Maurer, C., Schweigart, G. & Mergner, T. Pronounced overestimation of support surface tilt during stance. Exp Brain Res 168, 41–50 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-0087-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-005-0087-x

Keywords

Navigation