Skip to main content
Log in

Distributing vertical forces between the digits during gripping and lifting: the effects of rotating the hand versus rotating the object

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

During pinch grip we partition the vertical tangential forces at the digits according to the friction at the grip surfaces, and the mass distribution of the object. However, we cannot predictively partition the vertical forces to adjust to new frictional conditions after viewing a 180-deg rotation of an object with different textures at each grip surface. Hence, the processes that lead to predictive force partitioning may not access object representations, thereby suggesting that these processes are digit-specific. If this is true, then we should fail to predictively partition our fingertip forces when we rotate our hand. We tested this prediction by comparing the effects of object rotation with hand rotation for repeated lifts of an object that had one slippery grip surface and one rough grip surface. Subjects did not predictively redistribute the vertical tangential forces upon grasping the rotated object. Following object rotation, the vertical tangential force trajectories during the first 100 ms after contact indicated that 12/15 subjects failed to anticipate the reversed digit-friction relationships. All subjects appropriately partitioned the vertical tangential forces between the digits by the second lift after object rotation, confirming previous reports that sensory signals update the memory associated with lifting the object. In contrast, after hand rotation, 13/15 subjects anticipated the new digit-friction relationships and upon grasping the object immediately generated a steep rise in the vertical force trajectory at the rough surface. They also delayed the initial rise in vertical tangential force at the digit encountering the low-friction surface by approximately 65 ms. Thus, anticipatory partitioning of vertical fingertip forces is not strictly digit-specific. Internally driven motor plans can access the relevant memories or internal models for predictively partitioning the vertical tangential forces. It is not clear if this process involves rotating internal representations of fingertip force directly, or if the forces are derived after internally rotating a representation of the object. In contrast to the robust effects of vision on reach kinematics, or on wrist and finger configuration, visual signals about object rotation and orientation apparently do not influence vertical tangential fingertip forces.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 3A, B.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 5A, B.
Fig. 6.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arbib M (1981) Perceptual structures and distributed motor control. In: Brooks V (ed) Handbook of physiology: section 1. The Nervous System, Volume II, Motor Control, Part II. American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD, pp 1449–1480

  • Cole K, Rotella D (2002) Old age impairs the use of arbitrary visual cues for predictive control of fingertip forces during grasp. Exp Brain Res 143:35–41

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole K, Rotella D, Harper J (1998) Tactile impairments cannot explain the effect of age on a grasp and lift task. Exp Brain Res 121:263–269

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole K, Rotella D, Harper J (1999) Mechanisms for age-related changes of fingertip forces during precision gripping and lifting in adults. J Neurosci 19:3238–3247

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edin BB, Westling G, Johansson RS (1992) Independent control of human finger-tip forces at individual digits during precision lifting. J Physiol 450:547–564

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentile A (1998) Implicit and explicit processes during acquisition of functional skills. Scand J Occup Ther 5:7–17

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos A, Massey J (1987) Cognitive spatial-motor processes. 1. The making of movements at various angles from a stimulus direction. Exp Brain Res 65:361–370

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos A, Lurito J, Petrides M, Schwartz A, Massey J (1989) Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science 243:234–236

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon A, Forssberg H, Johansson R, Westling G (1991a) Integration of sensory information during the programming of precision grip: comments on the contributions of size cues. Exp Brain Res 85:226–229

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon A, Forssberg H, Johansson R, Westling G (1991b) Visual size cues in the programming of manipulative forces during precision grip. Exp Brain Res 83:477–482

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon A, Westling G, Cole K, Johansson R (1993) Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. J Neurophysiol 69:1789–1796

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon A, Forssberg H, Iwasaki N (1994) Formation and lateralization of internal representations underlying motor commands during precision grip. Neurophychologia 32:555–568

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Haffenden AM, Goodale MA (2002) Learned perceptual associations influence visuomotor programming under limited conditions: cues as surface patterns. Exp Brain Res 147:473–484

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iberall T, Arbib MA (1990) Schemas for the control of hand movements: an essay on cortical localization. In: Goodale MA (ed) Vision and action: the control of grasping. Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp 163–180

  • Iberall T, Binhanm G, Arbib MA (1986) Opposition space as a structuring concept for the analysis of skilled hand movements. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York

  • Jeannerod M (1986) The formation of finger grip during prehension. A cortically mediated visuomotor pattern. Behav Brain Res 19:99–116

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M, Decety J (1990) The accuracy of visuomotor transformation. An investigation into the mechanisms of visual recognition of objects. Ablex Publishing Corporation, NJ

  • Jeannerod M, Arbib M, Rizzolatti G, Sakata H (1995) Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. TINS 18:314–320

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jenmalm P, Johansson R (1997) Visual and somatosensory information about object shape control manipulative fingertip forces. J Neurosci 17:4486–4499

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson RS, Westling G (1984) Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Exp Brain Res 56:550–564

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karnath H, Ferber S, Bulthoff H (2000) Neuronal representation of object orientation. Neurophychologia 38:1235–1241

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opinion in Neurobiol 9:718–727

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krouchev N, Kalaska J (2003) Context-dependent anticipation of different task dynamics: rapid recall of appropriate motor skills using visual cues.

  • MacKenzie C, Iberall T (1994) The grasping hand. North-Holland, New York

  • Murata A, Gallese V, Luppino G, Kaseda M, Sakata H (2000) Selectivity for the shape, size and orientation of objects for grasping in neurons of the monkey parietal area AIP. J Neurophysiol 83:2580–2601

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura T, Kuroda T, Wakita M, Kusunoki M, Kato A, Mikami A, Sakata H, Itoh K (2001) From three-dimensional space vision to prehensile hand movements: the lateral intraparietal area links the area V3A and the anterior intraparietal area in macaques. J Neurosci 21:8174–8187

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parsons L, Gabrieli J, Phelps E, Gazzaniga M (1998) Cerebrally lateralized mental representations of hand shape and movement. J Neurosci 18:6539–6548

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quaney B, Rotella D, Peterson C, Cole KJ (2003) ‘Sensorimotor memory’ for fingertip forces: evidence for a task-independent motor memory. J Neurosci 23:1981–1986

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2002) Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 12:149–154

    Google Scholar 

  • Sakata H, Taira M, Kusunoki M, Tsutsui K, Tanaka Y, Shein WN, Miyashita Y (1999) Neural representation of three-dimensional features of manipulation objects with stereopsis. Exp Brain Res 128:160–169

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salimi I, Hollender I, Frazier W, Gordon A (2000) Specificity of internal representations underlying grasp. J Neurophysiol 84:2390–2397

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salimi I, Frazier W, Reilmann R, Gordon AM (2003) Selective use of visual information signaling objects’ center of mass for anticipatory control of manipulative fingertip forces. Exp Brain Res 150:9–18

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sinnaeve A, Dubrowski A, Carnahan H (2002) Evidence for the use of both iconic and long term memory systems for friction when grasping. Society Neurosci Abstract

  • Wada Y, Kawabata Y, Kotosaka S, Yamamoto K, Kitazawa S, Kawato M (2003) Acquisition and contextual switching of multiple internal models for different viscous force fields. Neurosci Res 46:319–331

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zatsiorsky VM, Gao F, Latash ML (2003) Prehension synergies: effects of object geometry and prescribed torques. Exp Brain Res 148:77–87

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a doctoral scholarship to the first author from the Foundation for Physical Therapy, Inc.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barbara M. Quaney.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Quaney, B.M., Cole, K.J. Distributing vertical forces between the digits during gripping and lifting: the effects of rotating the hand versus rotating the object. Exp Brain Res 155, 145–155 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1711-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1711-2

Keywords

Navigation