Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Trouble on my mind: the effect of catastrophic events on people’s worries

  • Published:
Empirical Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Major economic, environmental, or social shocks induce uncertainty, which in turn may impact economic development and may require institutional change. Based on the idea that catastrophic events (CEs) affect people’s perceptions of reality and judgments about the future, this paper analyzes the effect of CEs on people’s worries in terms of social, economic, and environmental issues. In particular, we consider the terrorist attack 9/11 in 2001, the beginning of the financial crisis in 2008, and the nuclear disaster in Fukushima in 2011. We propose two possible mechanisms: A CE in one sphere may affect people’s worries in general (“spillover”) or it may lead to people focusing on that sphere and being less worried about other spheres (“crowding out”). We argue that the determinants of the mechanisms are related to the type of CE, that a person’s professional background moderates the influence of a CE on his or her worries, and that the subsequent development of worries is affected by whether institutional responses are contested. The analysis is based on longitudinal data of the German Socio-Economic Panel.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This approach, of course, causes some uncertainty as to whether people were already aware of the CE at the time of the interview. We check this assumption in a robustness test in “Appendix B”.

  2. This facilitates the interpretation of interaction effects in our econometric model stated in Eq. (1) below, cf. the discussion in Sect. 4.

  3. By contrast, a long-term general linear trend spanning the entire sample period from 1999 to 2013 will suffer from shifts and breaks of linearity due to a mixture of external influences on people’s worries. In addition, the assumption of time-constant unobserved heterogeneity can no longer be justified. Further, restricting the analysis to those individuals continuously surveyed for 15 years would substantially reduce sample size, cf. “Appendix B”.

  4. Note that the variance of the daily worry averages increases in the course of each calendar year according to the clustering of interview dates towards the beginning of the year.

  5. The nomenclature will soon become clear later on in this section.

  6. Note that the quadratic model adequately reflects the apparent baseline reversion seen in Fig. 1 only for limited time periods. Our limitation to two and a half years after the CE may be regarded as adequate, cf. Allison (1994) for a discussion.

  7. For the sake of clarity our notation implies a slight redundancy at this point. Formally, worries \( y_{it,v \cdot } \) may be attributed to a certain CE w only tentatively by relying on temporal proximity. To put it simply, we do not know the source, i.e. the trigger-CE of a person’s worries, cf. the discussion at the end of Sect. 3. We still stick with the index w in \( y_{it,vw} \) as it serves to distinguish the models for different CEs.

  8. This assumption will be relaxed in Sect. 5 by panel-robust statistical inference.

  9. Note that, in spite of the individual effects \( \alpha_{i} \) included in the above equation, the errors \( \varepsilon_{it} \) are potentially correlated over t for a given individual i (serial correlation), and heteroscedastic. For this reason, panel-robust errors were specified in Stata.

  10. This number is based on an average environmental worry level of 1.09 as of 2001 (cf. Table 2) and the corresponding reduction of 0.182 units given by the \( \beta_{1} \) estimate.

  11. This number corresponds to the vertex of the simple quadratic equation involving the terms months and months2 (with coefficients \( \beta_{2} \) and \( \beta_{3} \), respectively). The x-coordinate of the vertex is given by \( {-}\beta_{2} /\left( {2\beta_{3} } \right) \), i.e. 11 months after the CE, for environmental worries. The worry level at this vertex is given by \( \beta_{1} - \beta_{2}^{2} /\left( {4\beta_{3} } \right) \). See also Lind and Mehlum (2010).

References

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jan Seidel.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendix A: Further results

Appendix B: Robustness checks

In order to test for sample attrition and entry effects over the entire 15-year period ranging from 1999 to 2013 (including the 2-year “9/11” preliminary period and the 2-year “Fukushima” follow-up period), we re-estimate the model using a complete sample, i.e., people interviewed in each of the 15 years. Results for the one-time level effects (\( \beta_{1} \), cf. Sect. 4) are included in Table 5, which excludes all other covariates for brevity. For the “9/11” CE, magnitude and significance levels of the coefficients do not change as compared to Table 3. The same holds true for “Lehman” and “Fukushima,” with two exceptions of changing significance levels. Hence, our results are fairly robust as to sample composition over time.

Next, we check sensitivity as to the exact interview date. We introduce an artificial error (normal distribution with μ = 0 and σ = 1) to the recorded date in order to attenuate potential dependencies between CE and interview date. Table 5 shows that results are virtually robust as compared to Table 3. In addition, we rerun our analysis after removing all interviews carried out on the particular day of the respective CE with special attention to the “Fukushima” CE, where a number of 90 interviews were conducted on March 11, 2011. Results in Table 5 show that no distorting effects are to be expected.

Further, we consider the influence of the chosen two-year follow-up period for each CE. We re-estimate the model using a one-year and a three-year follow-up scenario, respectively. As to the shortened one-year follow-up period, there are three marked discrepancies compared to our previous results. For the “9/11” CE the \( \beta_{1} \) level effect estimate loses its significance for environmental worries. As to the “Lehman” and “Fukushima” CEs, the estimates for social worries turn significant. We conclude that our model is most affected by a shortening of the follow-up period requiring some time to capture the nonlinear evolution of worries following a CE. As to the extended three-year follow-up period, however, results are largely in line with Table 3 (where two estimates marked by “NA” are not available due to numerical instability). All in all, a two-year follow-up period as applied in Sect. 5 appears to be a reasonable choice, as results tend to stabilize upon extension to a three-year follow-up, see also Goebel et al. (2014).

Next, note that the main effect (i.e., not interacted with CE) of (nearly) time-invariant variables such as sex or education is generally not identified in FE models (in contrast to time-varying interaction effects). As a robustness check we will include a random effects (RE) estimator instead of the FE estimator applied in Sect. 5. The RE level effects displayed in Table 5 are in broad agreement with the FE results of Table 3. Hence, we may avoid the more sensitive RE assumptions which, in addition, are clearly rejected by a Hausman test (also included in Table 5). See e.g., Wooldridge (2010) for further technical details and precise assumptions of the FE and RE approaches.

Finally, we test whether our findings are robust to an ordered probit model (using Stata’s xtoprobit) in order to challenge the cardinality assumption introduced in Sect. 4. Note that, due to the nonlinear structure of the probit model, only the coefficients’ sign and significance can be compared. With this in mind, results appear to be robust against the linear model. Due to numerical instability the result for economic worries following the Fukushima CE is not available.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ehlert, A., Seidel, J. & Weisenfeld, U. Trouble on my mind: the effect of catastrophic events on people’s worries. Empir Econ 59, 951–975 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00181-019-01682-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00181-019-01682-9

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation