Skip to main content
Log in

Shifts in Postdiscrimination Gradients Within a Stimulus Dimension Based on Female Waist-to-Hip Ratios

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Fifteen participants were trained on a within-subjects basis with two discrimination training conditions and one simple generalization training (control) condition to determine whether gradient shifts can be obtained within a stimulus dimension consisting of women’s waist-to-hip ratios (WHRs). In one discrimination condition, the S− consisted of the “optimal” WHR; in the other, the S− consisted of the approximate mean WHR for adult women. For all three conditions, the S+ was an intermediate value. Under both discrimination training conditions, the generalization gradient was observed to shift away from the S− and toward extreme values on the opposite end of the dimension; under the control condition, the gradient was more closely centered on the S+. The results suggest that the processes involved in gradient shifts can affect judgments of biologically significant stimuli.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BAERENDS, G. 1982. The herring gull and its egg. Part II. The responsiveness to egg-features. Behaviour, 82, 358–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • BRAY, G. A. (1992). Pathophysiology of obesity. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 55, 488S−494S.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BUSS, D. M. (1987). Sex differences in human mate selection criteria: An evolutionary perspective. In C. Crawford, M. Smith, & D. Krebs (Eds.), Sociobiology and psychology: Ideas, issues and applications (pp. 335–351). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • BUS, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypothesis tested in 37 cultures. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CHENG, K., & SPETCH, M. (2002). Spatial generalization and peak shift in humans. Learning and Motivation, 330, 358–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CHENG, K., SPETCH, M. L., & JOHNSTON, M. (1997). Spatial peak shift and generalization in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 469–481.

    Google Scholar 

  • CHENG, K., SPETCH, M. L., KELY, D. M., & BINGMAN, V. P. (2006). Small-scale spatial cognition in pigeons. Behavioural Processes, 72, 115–127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DERENE, A. (2006). Effects of S+, S− separation on gradient shifts in humans. Journal of General Psychology, 133, 163–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DERENE, A., & BREITSTEIN, R. M. (2006). Gradient shifts with naturally occurring human face stimuli. The Psychological Record, 56, 365–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DOBELSTEYN, C. J., JOFRES, M. R., MACLEAN, D. R., & FLOWERDEW, G. A. (2001). Comparative evaluation of waist circumference, waist-to-hip ratio, and body mass index as indicators of cardiovascular risk factors. The Canadian Heart Health Surveys. International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders, 25, 652–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FURNHAM, A., PETRIDES, K. V., & CONSTANTINIDES, A. (2005). The effects of body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio on ratings of female attractiveness, fecundity, and health. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1823–1834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GALIZIO, M. (1985). Human peak shift: Analysis of the effects of threestimulus discrimination training. Learning and Motivation, 16, 478–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAMBERALE, G., & TULLBERG, B. S. (1996). Evidence for peak-shift in predictor generalization among aposematic prey. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences, 263, 1329–1334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAMBERALE-STILLE, G., & TULBERG, B. S. (1999). Experienced chicks show biased avoidance of stronger signals: An experiment with natural colour variation in live aposematic prey. Evolutionary Ecology, 13, 579–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GHIRLANDA, S., & ENQUIST, M. (1998). Artificial neural networks as models of stimulus control. Animal Behaviour, 56, 1383–1389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GHIRLANDA, S., & ENQUIST, M. (1999). The geometry of stimulus control. Animal Behaviour, 58, 695–706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GHIRLANDA, S., & ENQUIST, M. (2003). A century of generalization. Animal Behaviour, 66, 15–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HANSON, H. M. (1959). Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 321–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • HENSS, R. (1995). Waist-to-hip ratio and attractiveness: Replication and extension. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 479–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HONIG, W. K., & URCUIOLI, P. J. (1981). The legacy of Guttman and Kalish (1956): 25 years of research on stimulus generalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 405–445.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • JENKINS, H. M., & HARISON, R. H. (1960). Effects of discrimination training on auditory generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 246–253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LAHTI-KOSKI, M., PIETINEN, P., MÄNNISTÖ, S., & VARTIAINEN, E. (2000). Trends in waist-to-hip ratio and its determinants in adults in Finland from 1987 to 1997. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 72, 1436–1444.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LEIMER, O., ENQUIST, M., & SILLÉN-TULBERG, B. (1986). Evolutionary stability of aposematic coloration and prey unprofitability: A theoretical analysis. American Naturalist, 128, 469–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LEVINE, M. P., & SMOLAK, L. (2006). The prevention of eating problems and eating disorders: Theory, research, and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • LEWIS, M. B., & JOHNSTON, R. A. (1999). Are caricatures special? Evidence of peak shift in face recognition. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 105–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LISSNER, L., BJÖRKELUND, C., HEITMAN, B. L., LAPIDUS, L., BJÖRNTORP, P., & BENGTSON, C. (1998). Secular increases in waist-hip ratio among Swedish women. International Journal of Obesity, 22, 1116–1120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MARLOWE, F., & WETSMAN, A. (2001). Preferred waist-to-hip ratio and ecology. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 481–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLAREN, I. P. L., BENET, C. H., GUTMAN-NAHIR, T., KIM, K., & MACKINTOSH, N. J. (1995). Prototype effects and peak shift in categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 662–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLAREN, I. P. L., & MACKINTOSH, N. J. (2002). Associative learning and elemental representation II: Generalization and discrimination. Animal Learning and Behavior, 30, 177–200.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’DONELL, J., CROSBIE, J., WILLIAMS, D. C., & SAUNDERS, K. J. (2000). Stimulus control and generalization of point-loss punishment with humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 73, 261–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PRICE, G. M., UAUY, R., BREZE, E., BULPITT, C. J., & FLETCHER, A. E. (2006). Weight, shape, and mortality risk in older persons: Elevated waist-hip ratio, not high body mass index, is associated with a greater risk of death. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 84, 449–460.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • RAMACHANDRAN, V. S., & HIRSTEIN, W. (1999). The science of art: A neurological theory of aesthetic experience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 15–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • REXRODE, K. M., CAREY, V. J., HENEKENS, C. H., WALTERS, E. E., COLDITZ, G. A., STAMPFER, M. J., WILLETT, W. C., & MANSON, J. E. (1998). Abdominal adiposity and coronary heart disease in women. Journal of the American Medical Association, 280, 1843–1848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • RILLING, M. (1977). Stimulus control and inhibitory processes. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 432–480). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • SEIDEL, J. C., PÉRUSSE, L., DESPRÉS, J.-P., & BOUCHARD, C. (2001). Waist and hip circumferences have independent and opposite effects on cardiovascular disease risk factors: The Quebec Family Study. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 74, 315–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • SINGH, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 293–307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SPETCH, M., CHENG, K., CLIFFORD, C. (2004). Peak shift but not range effects in recognition of faces. Learning and Motivation, 35, 221–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STADON, J. E. R. (1975). A note on the evolutionary significance of “supernormal” stimuli. The American Naturalist, 109, 541–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STREETER, S. A., & McBURNEY, D. H. (2003). Waist-hip ratio and attractiveness: New evidence and a critique of “a critical test.” Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 88–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SYMONS, D. (1995). Beauty is in the adaptations of the beholder: The evolutionary psychology of human female attractiveness. In P. R. Abramson & S. D. Pinkerton (Eds.), Sexual nature, sexual culture (pp. 80–118). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • THOMAS, D. R. (1993). A model for adaptation-level effects on stimulus generalization. Psychological Review, 100, 658–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • THOMAS, D. R., WINDELL, B. T., WILLIAMS, J. L., & WHITE, K. G. (1985). Stimulus presentation frequency in brightness discrimination and generalization: A test of adaptation-level and signal detection interpretations. Perception & Psychophysics, 37, 243–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WAS, P., WALDENSTRÖM, U., RÖSSNER, S., & HELBERG, D. (1997). An android body fat distribution in females impairs the pregnancy rate of in-vitro fertilization-embryo transfer. Human Reproduction, 12, 2057–2060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WEARY, D. M., GUILFORD, T. C., & WEISMAN, R. G. (1993). A product of discriminative learning may lead to female preferences for elaborate males. Evolution, 47, 333–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WEEDEN, J., & SABINI, J. (2005). Physical attractiveness and health in Western societies: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 635–653.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WILLS, S., & MACKINTOSH, N. J. (1998). Peak shift on an artificial dimension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51B, 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • YU, D. W., & SHEPARD, G. H. (1998). Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Nature, 396, 321–322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • ZIMMER, R. (2003). Abstraction in art. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, Biological Sciences, 358, 1285–1291.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adam Derenne.

Additional information

This material is based on work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 01322899. Thanks are extended to Brittany Bechthold, Karla Fehr, and Carmen Schanilec for assisting with data collection.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Derenne, A., Breitstein, R.M. & Cicha, R.J. Shifts in Postdiscrimination Gradients Within a Stimulus Dimension Based on Female Waist-to-Hip Ratios. Psychol Rec 58, 51–60 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395602

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395602

Navigation