Skip to main content
Log in

Delayed Matching to Sample: Probability of Responding in Accord with Equivalence as a Function of Different Delays

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present series of 4 experiments investigated the probability of responding in accord with equivalence in adult human participants as a function of increasing or decreasing delays in a many-to-one (MTO) or comparison-as-node and one-to-many (OTM) or sample-as-node conditional discrimination procedure. In Experiment 1, 12 participants started with simultaneous matching, followed by O-s, 2-s, and 4-s delayed matching, while the other 12 participants started with 4-s delayed matching, followed by the decreasing delays in a MTO training structure. Results suggested that the likelihood of responding in accord with equivalence is enhanced as a function of increasing delays during training, and also that starting with longer delays increases the probability of responding in accord with equivalence in simultaneous matching. In Experiment 2,6 participants started with simultaneous matching, followed by O-s, 2-s, and 4-s delayed matching in a OTM training structure. Results showed that 6 of 6 participants responded in accord with equivalence. In Experiment 3, 6 participants started with simultaneous matching, followed by O-s, 3-s, and 9-s delayed matching in a OTM training structure. Results showed that 6 of 6 participants responded in accord with equivalence. In Experiment 4, 6 participants started with simultaneous matching, followed by O-s and 3-s delayed matching with a restriction to engage in mediating behavior during testing. Results showed that none of the 6 participants responded in accord with equivalence during 3-s delay. It is argued that if delayed matching mquires (or evokes) precurrent behavior (responses that increase the effectiveness of a subsequent current behavior in obtaining a reinforcer), precurrent behavior may in turn facilitate responding in accord with equivalence. Higher reaction times durin~l initial testing may indicate precurrent problem-solving behavior prior to the selection of a comparison stimulus.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ARNTZEN, E. (2004). Probability of equivalence formation: Familiar stimuli and training sequence. The Psychological Record, 54, 275–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • ARNTZEN, E., & HOLTH, P. (1997). Probability of stimulus equivalence as a function of training design. The Psychological Record, 47, 309–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • ARNTZEN, E., & HOLTH, P. (2000a). Differential probabilities of equivalence outcome in individual subjects as a function of training structure. The Psychological Record, 50, 603–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ARNTZEN, E., & HOLTH, P. (2000b). Probability of stimulus equivalence as a function of class size vs. number of classes. The Psychological Record, 50, 79–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D. (1994). Stimulus equivalence and relational frame theory. The Psychological Record, 44, 91–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES, D., HEGARTY, N., & SMEETS, P. M. (1997). Relating equivalence relations to equivalence relations: A relational framing model complex human functioning. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 14, 57–83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BENTALL, R. P., DICKINS, D. W., & FOX, S. R. A. (1993). Naming and equivalence: Response latencies for emergent relations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 46B, 187–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • BLOUGH, D. (1959). Delayed matching in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2, 151–160.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BUFFINGTON, D. M., FIELDS, L, & ZDAMS, B. J. (1997). Enhancing equivalence class formation by pretraining of other equivalence classes. The Psychological Record, 47, 69–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CLEAVELAND, J. M. (1998). Stimulus properties of the response during matching-to-sample tasks in pigeons and budgerigars. Dissertation Abstracts International Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 59, 1833.

    Google Scholar 

  • CRITCHFIELD, T. S., & PERONE, M. (1993). Verbal self reports about matching to sample: Effects of the number of elements in a compound sample stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 193–214.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CUMMING, W. W., & BERRYMAN, R. (1965). The complex discriminated operant: Studies of matching-to-sample and related problems. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Stimulus generalization (pp. 284–330). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DE ROSE, J. C. (1996). Controlling factors in conditional discriminations and tests of equivalence. In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals (pp. 253–277). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • DONAHOE, J. W., & PALMER, D. C. (1994). Learning and complex behavior. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • DUGDALE, N., & LOWE, C. F. (1990). Naming and stimulus equivalence. In D. E. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behaviour analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 115–138). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • ERICSSON, K. A., & SIMON, H. A. (1984). Protocol analysis. Verbal reports as data. Cambridge, MA: The Mit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L, REEVE, K. F., ROSEN, D., VARELAS, A., ADAMS, B. J., BELANICH, J., et al. (1997). Using the simultaneous protocol to study equivalence class formation: The facilitating effects of nodal number and size of previously established equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 367–389.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FIELDS, L., REEVE, K. F., VARELAS, A., ROSEN, D., & BELANICH, J. (1997). Equivalence class formation using stimulus-pairing and yes-no. The Psychological Record, 47, 661–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GREEN, G., MACKAY, H. A., MCILVANE, W. J., SAUNDERS, R. R., & SORACI, S. A. (1990). Perspectives on relational learning in mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 95, 249–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • HARTL, J. A., DOUGHERTY, D. H., & WIXTED, J. T. (1996). Separating the effects of trial-specific and average sample-stimulus duration in delayed matching to sample in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 66, 231–242.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HARTL, J. A., & FANTINO, E. (1996). Choice as a function of reinforcement ratios in delayed matching to sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 66, 11–27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HEALY, O., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & SMEETS, P. M. (2000). Derived relational responding as generalized operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 207–227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HOLTH, P., & ZRNTZEN, E. (1998a). Stimulus familiarity and the delayed emergence of stimulus equivalence or consistent nonequivalence. The Psychological Record, 48, 81–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HOLTH, P., & ZRNTZEN, E. (1998b). Symmetry versus sequentiality related to prior training, sequential dependency of stimuli, and verbal labeling. The Psychological Record, 48, 293–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HOLTH, P., & ZRNTZEN, E. (2000). Reaction times and the emergence of class consistent responding: A case for precurrent responding? The Psychological Record, 50, 305–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • HORNE, P. J., & LOWE, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 181–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HOVE, O. (2003). Differential probability of equivalence class formation following a one-to-many versus a many-to-one training structure. The Psychological Record, 53, 617–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HUNTER, W. S. (1913). The delayed reaction in animals and children. American Journal of Psychology, 35, 86.

    Google Scholar 

  • IMAM, A. A. (2001). Speed contingencies, number of stimulus presentations, and the nodality effect in equivalence formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76, 265–288.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • JANS, J. J., & CATANIA, A. C. (1980). Short-term remembering of discriminative stimuli in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34, 177–183.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • JOHNSON, O, & SIDMAN, M. (1993). Conditional discrimination and equivalence relations: Control by negative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 333–347.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LANE, S. D., & CRITCHFIELD, T. S. (1996). Verbal self-reports of emergent relations in a stimulus equivalence procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 355–374.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LANE, S. D., & CRITCHFIELD, T. S. (1998). Classifications of vowels and consonants by individuals with moderate and mental retardation: Development of arbitrary relations via matching-to-sample training with compound stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 21–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LAZAR, R., DAVIS-LANG, D., & SANCHEZ, L. (1984). The formation of visual stimulus equivalence in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 251–266.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LOWE, C. F., & BEASTY, A. (1987). Language and the emergence of equivalence relations: A developmental study. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 40, A42.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’DONNELL, J., & SAUNDERS, K. J. (2003). Equivalence relations in individuals with language limitations and mental retardation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 80, 131–157.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • PALMER, D. C. (1991). A behavioral interpretation of memory. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 261–279). Reno, NY: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • PALMER, D. C. (2003). Cognition. In K. A. Lattai & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Behavior theory and philosophy (pp. 167–185). New York: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • PARSONS, J. A., & FERRARO, D. P. (1977). Complex interactions: A functional approach. In B. C. Etzel, J. M. Le Blanc, & D. M. Baer (Eds.), New developments in behavioral research: Theory, method, and application. In honor of Sidney W. Bijou (pp. 237–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • PARSONS, J. A., TAYLOR, D. O., & JOYCE, T. M. (1981). Precurrent self-prompting operants in children: Remembering. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 253–266.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • PAULE, M. G., BUSHNELL, P. J., MAURISSEN, J. P. J., WENGER, G. R., BUCCAFUSCO, J. J., CHELONIS, J. J., et al. (1998). Symposium overview: The use of delayed matching-to-sample procedures in studies of short-term memory in animals and humans. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 20, 493–502.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • POTTER, B., HUBER, S., & MICHAEL, J. (1997). The role of mediating verbal behavior in selection-based responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 14, 41–56.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • REHFELDT, A., & HAYES, L. J. (2000). The long-term retention of generalized equivalence. The Psychological Record, 50, 405–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • REHFELDT, A. R., DIXON, M. R., HAYES, L J., & STEELE, A. (1998). Stimulus equivalence and the blocking effect. The Psychological Record, 48, 647–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SARGISSON, R. J., & WHITE, K. G. (2001). Generalization of delayed matching to sample following training at different delays. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 1–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SATO, T. (2001). The function of common naming and establishment of stimulus equivalence. Japanese Journal of Behavior Analysis, 16, 2–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, K. J., SAUNDERS, R. R., WILLIAMS, D. O., & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1993). An interaction of instructions and training design on stimulus class formation: Extending the analysis of equivalence. The Psychological Record, 43, 725–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., DRAKE, K. M., & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1999). Equivalence class development, expansion, and modification in preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 195–214.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., & GREEN, G. (1999). A discrimination analysis of training structure effects on stimulus equivalence outcomes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 72, 117–137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., & MCENTEE, J. (2004). Increasing the probability of stimulus equivalence with adults with mild mental retardation. The Psychological Record, 54, 340–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • SAUNDERS, R. R., WACHTER, J. A., & SPRADLIN, J. E. (1988). Establishing auditory stimulus control over an eight-member equivalence class via conditional discrimination procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49, 95–115.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SHIMOFF, E. (1984). Post-session questionnaires. Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin, 2, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson & M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1990). Equivalence relations: Where do they come from? In D. E. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behaviour analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 93–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1992). Equivalence relations: Some basic considerations. In S. C. Hayes & L. J. Hayes (Eds.), Understanding verbal relations (pp. 15–27). Reno, NY: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., & TAILBY, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M., WILLSON-MORRIS, M., & KIRK, B. (1986). Matching-to-sample procedures and the development of equivalence relations: The role of naming. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1968). The technology of teaching. New York: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • SPENCER, T. J., & CHASE, P. N. (1996). Speed analysis of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 643–659.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SPRADLIN, J. E., & SAUNDERS, R. R. (1986). The development of stimulus classes using match-to-sample procedures: Sample classification versus comparison classification. Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 6, 41–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STOKES, T. F., & BAER, D. M. (2003). Mediated generalization: An unfinished portrait. In K. S. Budd & T. F. Stokes (Eds.), A small matter of proof: The legacy of Donald M. Baer (pp. 125–138). Reno, NY: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R., & MACKAY, H. A. (1992). Spelling and emergent picture-printed word relations established with delayed identity matching to complex samples. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 893–904.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • STROMER, R., & MACKAY, H. A. (1996). Naming and the formation of stimulus classes. In T. R. Zentall & P. M. Smeets (Eds.), Stimulus class formation in humans and animals (pp. 221–252). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • TORGRUD, L. J., & HOLBORN, S. W. (1989). Effectiveness and persistence of precurrent mediating behavior in delayed matching to sample and oddity matching with children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 181–191.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • URCUIOLI, P. J., DEMARSE, T. B., & LIONELLO, K. M. (1999). Sample-duration effects on pigeons’ delayed matching as a function of predictability of duration. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 72, 279–297.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • URCUIOLI, P. J., & ZENTALL, T. R. (1986). Retrospective coding in pigons’ delayed matching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 69–77.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WATSON, J. B. (1920). Is thinking merely the action of language mechanism? British Journal of Psychology, 11, 87–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • WIRTH, O., & CHASE, P. N. (2002). Stability of functional equivalence and stimulus equivalence: Effects of baseline reversals. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 77, 29–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WIXTED, J. T. (1989a). Nonhuman short-term memory: A quantitative reanalysis of selected findings. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 409–426.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WIXTED, J. T. (1989b). The vocabulary of remembering: A review of Kendrick, Rilling, and Denny’s Theories of Animal Memory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 441–450.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WULFERT, E., DOUGHER, M. J., & GREENWAY, D. E. (1991). Protocol analysis of the correspondence of verbal behavior and equivalence class formation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 489–504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erik Arntzen.

Additional information

Parts of the current study were presented as a paper at the ABA convention in San Francisco in 2003.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Arntzen, E. Delayed Matching to Sample: Probability of Responding in Accord with Equivalence as a Function of Different Delays. Psychol Rec 56, 135–167 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395541

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395541

Navigation