Skip to main content
Log in

Causality and Contingency: Some Conceptual Considerations

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The logical features of causality (necessary connection, succession, and contiguity) are examined in regard to respondent and operant behaviors. It is emphasized that operant behavior as emitted behavior breaks with any criterion of precedent causality. It is examined how Skinner tried to approach causality as functional relation, without overcoming a pure phenomenological analysis. The category of contingency is submitted to a conceptual analysis, and its application to superstition, respondent and operant conditioning is critically reviewed. A reinterpretation of contingency as conditionality is proposed, and the analysis of the organization of behavior is advocated in terms of interdependent functional and occurrence contingencies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BEECROFT, R. S. (1966). Classical conditioning. Goleta: Psychonomic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • BEKHTEREW, W. von. (1913). Objektive Psychologie oder Psychoreflexologie. Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • BROWN, P. L., … JENKINS, H. M. (1968). Autoshaping of the pigeon’s key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DESCARTES, R. (1986, Spanish translation). Ei Mundo o Tratado de la Luz. Mexico: Unam.

    Google Scholar 

  • FARMER, J., … SCHOENFELD, W. N. (1966a). Varying temporal placement of an added stimulus in a fixed-interval reinforcement schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 369–375.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FARMER, J., … SCHOENFELD, W. N. (1966b). The effect of a responsecontingent stimulus introduced into a fixed-interval schedule at varying temporal placement. Psychonomic Science, 6, 15–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FERSTER, C. B. (1958). Intermittent reinforcement of a complex response in a chimpanzee. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 1, 163–165.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GUTHRIE, E. W. (1935). The psychology of learning. New York: Harper … Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • HERRNSTEIN, R. J. (1966). Superstition: A corollary of operant conditioning. In W. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Research and applications (pp. 33–51). New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • HUME, D. (1977, Spanish translation). Tratado de la Naturaleza Humana. Mexico: Porrua.

    Google Scholar 

  • MERLEAU-PONTY, M. (1976, Spanish translation). La estructura del comportamiento. Buenos Aires: Hachette.

    Google Scholar 

  • PAVLOV, I. P. (1927, English translation). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • PREMACK, D. (1962). Reversibility of the reinforcement relation. Science, 136, 255–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • RAZRAN, G. (1971). Mind in evolution. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • RIBES, E. (1986). Language as behavior: Functional mediation vs. morphological description. In H. W. Reese … L. Parrott (Eds.), Behavior science: Philosophical, methodological and empirical advances (pp. 115–138). Hillsdale, Nj: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • RIBES, E. (1991). Language as contingency-substitution behavior. In L. J. Hayes … P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogue on verbal behavior (pp. 47–63). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • RIBES, E. (1993a). An analysis of thinking. In S. C. Hayes … L. J. Hayes (Eds.), Understanding verbal relations (pp. 209–224). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • RIBES, E. (1993b). Behavior as the functional content of language games. In S. C. Hayes, L. J. Hayes, H. W. Reese, … T. R. Sarbin (Eds.), Varieties of scientific contextualism (pp. 283–297). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • RIBES, E., … CHAVEZ, R. (1988). Efectos de la comida no contingente en la conducta libre de la rata blanca: consideraciones sobre el analisis del segmento de respuesta. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 14, 247–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • RYLE, G. (1949). The concept of mind. New York: Barnes … Noble.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHLINGER, H. D. (1993). Separating discriminative and function-altering effects of verbal stimuli. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 9–23.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SCHOENFELD, W. N., … COLE, B. K. (1972). Stimulus schedules: The T-t systems. New York: Harper … Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHOENFELD, W. N., … FARMER, J. (1970). Reinforcement schedules and the behavior stream. In W. N. Schoenfeld (Ed.), The theory of reinforcement schedules. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHOENFELD, W. N., COLE, B. K., LANG, J., … MANKOFF, R. “Contingency” in behavior theory. In F. J. McGuigan … D. B. Lumsden (Eds.), Contemporary approaches to conditioning and learning. New York: Winston.

  • SECHENOV, I. (1866, Russian original, 1978-Spanish translation). Los ref/ejos del cerebro. Barcelona: Fontanella.

    Google Scholar 

  • SHAPIRO, M. M. (1960). Salivary respondent conditioning during lever-pressing in dogs. Science, 132, 619–620.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent verbal classes. In T. Thompson … M. D. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research history. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1935). The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. The Journal of General Psychology, 12, 40–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1937). Two types of conditioned reflex: A reply to Konorski and Miller. The Journal of General Psychology, 16, 264–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1948). “Superstition” in the pigeon. The Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 168–172.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • STADDON, J. E. R., … SIMMELHAG, V. (1971). The superstition experiment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. The Psychological Review, 78, 3–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STADDON, J. E. R., … AYRES, S. (1975). Sequential and temporal properties of behavior induced by a schedule of periodic food delivery. Behaviour, 54, 26–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • TERRACE, H. S. (1966). Stimulus control. In W. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 271–344). New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emilio Ribes-Iñesta.

Additional information

Reprint requests may be sent to Emilio Ribes-Inesta, Centro de Estudios e Investigaciones en Comportamiento, 12 de diciembre 204 (Col. Chapalita), Guadalajara, Jal. 45020 (Mexico). Email: ribes@udgserv.cencar.udg.mx

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ribes-Iñesta, E. Causality and Contingency: Some Conceptual Considerations. Psychol Rec 47, 619–635 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395249

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395249

Navigation