Skip to main content
Log in

An Animal Analogue of Gambling

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Pigeons were trained in a gambling analogue procedure. At the start of a trial a bird was faced with a choice between a present “sure thing” (FR 30) and the opportunity to acquire a better condition of reinforcement (one or more FR 10s). The smaller ratios were given probabilistically following the gamble response. If the lower ratio did not occur a 1-min time-out was given. The FR 30 or FR 10 schedules were followed by time-out. The results show that pigeons chose the risky alternative more when the probability of FR 10 was high (.5) but the number of FRs given was small (one) than when the probability of a win was small (.1) with five FRs. An intermediate combination was similar to the.5 condition. In addition it was shown that when the chance of a win was small, immediate response contingent information about winning was important in maintaining choice for the risky alternative. In all conditions where the birds chose to gamble session length was substantially longer than under FR 30 without the opportunity to gamble.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • CARACO, T., MARTINDALE, S., & WHITMAN, T. S. (1980). An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behavior, 28, 820–830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • COLLIER, G. H., JOHNSON, D. F., HILL, W. L, & KAUFMAN, L. W. (1986). The economics of the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 113–136.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CROSSMAN, E. (1983). Las Vegas knows better. The Behavior Analyst, 6, 109–110.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DICKERSON, M. G. (1984). Compulsive gamblers. London; Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • FANTINO, DUNN, & MECK (1979).

  • FERSTER, C. B., & SKINNER, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • HERRNSTEIN, R. J. (1964). Aperiodicity as a factor in choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7, 179–182.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HURSH, (1980).

  • KILLEEN, P. (1968). On the measurement of reinforcement frequency in the study of preference. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 263–269.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KENDALL, S. B. (1974). Preference for intermittent reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 463–473.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KENDALL, S. B. (1985). Conditioned reinforcement based on multiple primary reinforcers. The Psychological Record, 36, 215–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • KENDALL, S. B. (1986). A further study of choice and percentage reinforcement. Behavioural Processes, 10, 399–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LEVENTHAL, A. M., MORRELL, R. F., MORGAN, E. F., Jr., & PERKINS, C. C, Jr. (1959). The relation between mean reward and mean reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 280–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LOGUE, A. W., SMITH, M. E., & RACHLIN, H. (1985). Sensitivity of pigeons to prereinforcer and postreinforcer delay. Animal Learning and Behavior, 13, 181–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MAZUR, J. E. (1985). Probability and delay of reinforcement as factors in discrete trial choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 341–351.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MAZUR, J. E. (1986). Choice between single and multiple delayed reinforcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 67–77.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MOORE, J. (1985). Choice and the conditioned reinforcing strength of informative stimuli. The Psychological Record, 35, 89–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • MORRIS, C. J. (1986). The effects of occasional (FR 1) reinforcement ratios on choice behavior. The Psychological Record, 36, 63–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • MOUNTJOY, P. (1985, May). What gamblers could have taught behavior analysts about stimulus and schedule of behavior. Invited address given to the Association for Behavior Analysis, Columbus, Ohio.

    Google Scholar 

  • NAVARICK, D. J., & FANTINO, E. (1972). Transitivity as a property of choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, 389–401.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • RACHLIN, H., & GREEN, I. (1972). Commitment, choice and self control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 15–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SHULL, R. L., SPEAK, D. J., & BRYSON, A. E. (1981). Delay or rate of food delivery as a determiner of response rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 119–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kendall, S.B. An Animal Analogue of Gambling. Psychol Rec 37, 247–256 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394987

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03394987

Navigation