Skip to main content
Log in

Defining Delayed Consequences as Reinforcers: Some Do, Some Don’t, and Nothing Changes

  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Results of a survey sent to members of the editorial boards of five behavior-analytic journals in 1990 indicated that there was no consensus among respondents with respect to whether delayed events can function as reinforcers (Schlinger, Blakely, Fillhard, amp; Poling, 1991). Since that time, several studies with nonhuman animals have demonstrated that events delayed by at least several seconds can increase the probability of occurrence of the responses that produce them. To determine the relative number of contemporary behavior analysts who nonetheless believe that reinforcers must immediately follow the responses that produce them, the present study replicated the procedures used by Schlinger et al., with similar results. That is, respondents did not agree with respect to whether delayed events could be reinforcers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alberto, P. A., & Troutman, A. C. (2009). Applied behavior analysis for teachers (8th ed.). New York: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anger, D. (1963). The role of temporal discriminations in the reinforcement of Sidman avoidance behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 477–506.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bachmann, D. P., Elfrink, J., & Vazzana, G. (2000). E-mail and snail mail face off in rematch. Marketing Research, 11, 10–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chance, P. (2006). Learning and behavior (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Thomson Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cobanoglu, C., Warde, B., & Moreo, P. J. (2001). A comparison of mail, fax, and web-base survey methods. International Journal of Market Research, 43, 441–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T. S., & Kollins, S. H. (2001). Temporal discounting: Basic research and the analysis of socially important behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 101–122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T. S., & Lattal, K. A. (1993). Acquisition of a spatially defined operant with delayed reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 373–387.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E., & Logan, C. A. (1979). The experimental analysis of behavior. A biological perspective. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 767–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Hineline, P. N. (1966). Negative reinforcement as shock-frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 421–430.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. M., & Pennypacker, H. S. (2009). Strategies and tactics of behavioral research (3rd ed.). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Behavior modification in applied settings. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Thomson Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A., & Gleeson, S. (1990). Response acquisition with delayed reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 27–39.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lattal, K. A., & Metzger, B. (1994). Response acquisition by Siamese fighting fish (Beta splendens) with delayed visual reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 61, 35–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, J. C., & O’Reilly, M. F. (2003). Behavior analysis: Foundations and applications to psychology. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malott, R. W. (2008). Principles of behavior analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, G., & Pear, J. J. (2007). Behavior modification: What it is and how to do it (8th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merriam-Webster’s Collegiate Dictionary. (2009). Immediately. Retrieved from http://www.merriam-webster.com/

  • Miltenberger, R. G. (2008). Behavior modification: Principles and procedures (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Thomson Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. (1995). Some historical and conceptual relations among logical positivism, behaviorism, and cognitive psychology. In J. T. Todd & E. K. Morris (Eds.), Modern perspectives on B. F. Skinner and contemporary behaviorism (pp. 51–74). Westport, CT: Greenwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. (2008). Conceptual foundations of radical behaviorism. Cornwall on Hudson, NY: Sloan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oxford English Dictionary. (2009). Immediately. Retrieved from http://www.oed.com/

  • Pear, J. J. (2001). The science of learning. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, W. D., & Cheney, C. D. (2008). Behavior analysis and learning (4th ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, L., Reeve, K. F., Brown, A. K., Brown, J. L., & Poulson, C. L. (1992). Effects of delayed reinforcement on infant vocalization rate. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Roll, J. M., Reilly, M. P., & Johanson, C. E. (2000). The influence of exchange delays on cigarette versus money choice: A laboratory analog of voucher-based reinforcement therapy. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8, 366–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarafino, E. P. (2004). Behavior modification: Principles of behavior change. Long Grove, IL: Waveland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlinger, H. D., & Blakely, E. (1994). The effects of delayed reinforcement and a response-produced auditory stimulus on the acquisition of operant behavior in rats. The Psychological Record, 44, 391–409.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlinger, H. D., Blakely, E., Fillhard, J., amp; Poling, A. (1991). Defining terms in behavior analysis: Reinforcer and discriminative stimulus. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 153–161.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schlinger, H. D., & Poling, A. (1998). Introduction to scientific psychology. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review, 52, 268–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stromer, R., McComas, J. J., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Designing interventions that include delayed reinforcement: Implications of recent laboratory research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 359–371.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sutphin, G., Byrne, T., & Poling, A. (1998). Response acquisition with delayed reinforcement: A comparison of two-lever procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 17–28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkenfield, J., Nickel, M., Blakely, E., amp; Poling, A. (1992). Acquisition of leverpress responding in rats with delayed reinforcement: A comparison of three procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 431–443.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, A. M., & Lattal, K. A. (1999). The role of the response-reinforcer relation in delay-of-reinforcement effects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 187–194.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alan Poling.

Additional information

The reported data were collected as part of the master’s thesis of Kelly Bradley. We thank all respondents for their help.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bradley, K.P., Poling, A. Defining Delayed Consequences as Reinforcers: Some Do, Some Don’t, and Nothing Changes. Analysis Verbal Behav 26, 41–49 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393081

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393081

Key words

Navigation